Immersive 360∘\documentclass[12pt]{minimal} \usepackage{amsmath} \usepackage{wasysym} \usepackage{amsfonts} \usepackage{amssymb} \usepackage{amsbsy} \usepackage{mathrsfs} \usepackage{upgreek} \setlength{\oddsidemargin}{-69pt} \begin{document}$$360^{\circ }$$\end{document} video user experience: impa

Virtual Reality (VR) has been recently gaining interest from researchers and companies, contributing to the development of the associated technologies that aim to transport its users to a virtual environment by the stimulation of their senses. Technologies such as Head-Mounted Displays (HMD), capable of presenting 360° video in 3D, are becoming affordable and, consequently, more common among the average consumer, potentiating the creation of a market for VR experiences. The purpose of this study is to measure the influence of (a) video format (2D/monoscopic vs 3D/stereoscopic), (b) sound format (2D/stereo vs 3D/spatialized), and (c) gender on users’ sense of presence and cybersickness, while experiencing a VR application using an HMD. Presence and cybersickness were measured using questionnaires as subjective measures. Portuguese versions of the Igroup Presence Questionnaire for presence and the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire for cybersickness were used. Results revealed no statistically significant differences between (a) VIDEO and (b) SOUND variables on both senses of presence and cybersickness. When paired with (a) VIDEO, the independent variable (c) Gender showed significant differences on almost all subscales of presence. Results suggest that the widely acknowledged differences in spatial ability between genders were a major factor contributing to this outcome.

[1]  Jari Takatalo,et al.  User Experience in 3D Stereoscopic Games , 2011 .

[2]  OmniPres project IST-2001-39237 Deliverable 5 Measuring Presence : A Guide to Current Measurement Approaches , 2004 .

[3]  António Augusto de Sousa,et al.  Adaptation and Validation of the Igroup Presence Questionnaire (IPQ) in a Portuguese Sample , 2016, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[4]  M. Rosa,et al.  Presence and emotions in virtual environments: the influence of stereoscopy. , 2008 .

[5]  Holger Regenbrecht,et al.  The Experience of Presence: Factor Analytic Insights , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[6]  Michael P. Snow,et al.  Empirical Models Based on Free-Modulus Magnitude Estimation of Perceived Presence in Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[7]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Using Behavioral Realism to Estimate Presence: A Study of the Utility of Postural Responses to Motion Stimuli , 2000, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[8]  Angelos Amditis,et al.  Human centred design of 3-D interaction devices to control virtual environments , 2006, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[9]  Dawn M. Sarno The Effect of Stereoscopic (3D) Movies on Psychological and Physiological Experiences , 2015 .

[10]  Rosa María Baños,et al.  Presence and Emotions in Virtual Environments: The Influence of Stereoscopy , 2008, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[11]  Darren George,et al.  SPSS for Windows Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference , 1998 .

[12]  Mel Slater,et al.  Depth of Presence in Virtual Environments , 1994, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[13]  Jonathan Freeman,et al.  Here, there and everywhere: The effects of multichannel audio on presence , 2001 .

[14]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  A Review of Presence and Performance in Virtual Environments , 2000, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[15]  David Kim,et al.  Vermeer: direct interaction with a 360° viewable 3D display , 2011, UIST.

[16]  Stephen Palmisano,et al.  Consistent Stereoscopic Information Increases the Perceived Speed of Vection in Depth , 2002, Perception.

[17]  Mark Billinghurst,et al.  Simulator Sickness in Mobile Spatial Sound Spaces , 2009, CMMR/ICAD.

[18]  R. Brislin Back-Translation for Cross-Cultural Research , 1970 .

[19]  Jari Takatalo,et al.  Measuring stereoscopic image quality experience with interpretation based quality methodology , 2008, Electronic Imaging.

[20]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  The Sense of Presence within Auditory Virtual Environments , 1996, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[21]  April L. Zenisky,et al.  Translating and Adapting Tests for Cross-Cultural Assessments , 2010 .

[22]  Michael E. McCauley,et al.  Cybersickness: Perception of Self-Motion in Virtual Environments , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[23]  Maria del Carmen Juan Lizandra,et al.  Comparison of the Levels of Presence and Anxiety in an Acrophobic Environment Viewed via HMD or CAVE , 2009, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[24]  Sumio Yano,et al.  Visual fatigue caused by stereoscopic images and the search for the requirement to prevent them: A review , 2012, Displays.

[25]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Effects of Stereoscopic Presentation, Image Motion, and Screen Size on Subjective and Objective Corroborative Measures of Presence , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[26]  W. Bles,et al.  Motion sickness. , 2000, Current opinion in neurology.

[27]  Dheva Raja,et al.  The Effects of Immersion on 3D Information Visualization , 2006 .

[28]  Wa Wijnand IJsselsteijn,et al.  Visual discomfort in stereoscopic displays: a review , 2007, Electronic Imaging.

[29]  Aamir Saeed Malik,et al.  Does 3D produce more symptoms of visually induced motion sickness? , 2013, 2013 35th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC).

[30]  Geoffrey S. Hubona,et al.  The relative contributions of stereo, lighting, and background scenes in promoting 3D depth visualization , 1999, TCHI.

[31]  Michael J. Singer,et al.  Measuring Presence in Virtual Environments: A Presence Questionnaire , 1998, Presence.

[32]  Zygmunt Pizlo,et al.  Design of studies to test the effectiveness of stereo imaging truth or dare: is stereo viewing really better? , 1994, Electronic Imaging.

[33]  Jennifer Lawrence,et al.  Communication in the Age of Virtual Reality. , 1997 .

[34]  Mel Slater,et al.  Immersion, presence and performance in virtual environments: an experiment with tri-dimensional chess , 1996, VRST.

[35]  E. Coluccia,et al.  Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review , 2004 .

[36]  Philip J. Corriveau,et al.  Psychovisual aspects of viewing stereoscopic video sequences , 1998, Electronic Imaging.

[37]  Mark Billinghurst,et al.  Occurrence of Simulator Sickness in Spatial Sound Spaces and 3D Auditory Displays , 2009 .

[38]  Rosa María Baños,et al.  Immersion and Emotion: Their Impact on the Sense of Presence , 2004, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[39]  Eugenia M. Kolasinski,et al.  Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments. , 1995 .

[40]  M. Linn,et al.  Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.

[41]  Ian Burnett,et al.  3D fatigue from stereoscopic 3D video displays: Comparing objective and subjective tests using electroencephalography , 2015, TENCON 2015 - 2015 IEEE Region 10 Conference.

[42]  Willem-Paul Brinkman,et al.  The Effect Of 3D Audio And Other Audio Techniques On Virtual Reality Experience. , 2015, Studies in health technology and informatics.

[43]  Albert A. Rizzo,et al.  Comparison of Two VR Platforms for Rehabilitation: Video Capture versus HMD , 2005, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[44]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  The Subjective Sense of Presence, Emotion Recognition, and Experienced Emotions in Auditory Virtual Environments , 2003, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[45]  P. Larsson,et al.  1 When What You Hear is What You See : Presence and Auditory-Visual Integration in Virtual Environments , 2007 .

[46]  Lizzy Bleumers,et al.  Seeing the bigger picture: a user perspective on 360° TV , 2012, EuroITV.

[47]  Mel Slater,et al.  Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  Joseph J. LaViola,et al.  Evaluating the benefits of 3d stereo in modern video games , 2011, CHI.

[49]  Thomas B. Sheridan,et al.  Musings on Telepresence and Virtual Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[50]  Wijnand A. IJsselsteijn,et al.  Presence: concept, determinants, and measurement , 2000, Electronic Imaging.

[51]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[52]  H. Hecht,et al.  Visually induced motion sickness and presence in videogames: The role of sound , 2012 .

[53]  Joseph J. LaViola,et al.  Understanding user experience in stereoscopic 3D games , 2012, CHI.

[54]  Luc Van Gool,et al.  Blue-c: a spatially immersive display and 3D video portal for telepresence , 2003, IPT/EGVE.

[55]  Jonathan Freeman,et al.  Really hear? The effects of audio quality on presence , 2001 .

[56]  John A. List,et al.  Nurture affects gender differences in spatial abilities , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Andreas Kunz,et al.  blue-c: a spatially immersive display and 3D video portal for telepresence , 2003, ACM Trans. Graph..

[58]  Matthew Lombard,et al.  At the Heart of It All: The Concept of Presence , 2006 .

[59]  Xiaoyu Zhang,et al.  Quantifying the benefits of immersion for collaboration in virtual environments , 2005, VRST '05.

[60]  P. Larsson,et al.  When what you see is what you hear: Auditory-visual integration and presence in virtual environments , 2005 .

[61]  Doug A. Bowman,et al.  Virtual Reality: How Much Immersion Is Enough? , 2007, Computer.

[62]  Joseph J. LaViola,et al.  A discussion of cybersickness in virtual environments , 2000, SGCH.

[63]  Rick Kazman,et al.  Using 3D sound as a navigational aid in virtual environments , 2004, Behav. Inf. Technol..