The Motor Locus of No-Go Backward Crosstalk
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] M. C. Smith,et al. The Psychological Refractory Period as a Function of Performance of a First Response , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[3] Ann Reynolds,et al. The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Richard Schweickert,et al. A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .
[5] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[6] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. Dissociating interference-control processes between memory and response. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[7] R W Proctor,et al. Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] C. Brunia. Motor preparation: changes in amplitude of Achilles tendon reflexes during a fixed foreperiod of one second. , 1983, Psychophysiology.
[9] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[10] F. Donders. On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.
[11] Scott Watter. Parallel Response Selection in Dual -Tasks , 2003 .
[12] Markus Janczyk,et al. Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[13] Jeff Miller,et al. Psychophysiological Measurement of Backward Response Activation in the Prioritized Processing Paradigm , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[15] Markus Janczyk,et al. Sequential modulation of backward crosstalk and task-shielding in dual-tasking. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Scott Watter,et al. PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] Torsten Schubert,et al. Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[19] Wilfried Kunde,et al. The locus of tool-transformation costs. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Markus Janczyk,et al. Action effect features, but not anatomical features, determine the Backward Crosstalk Effect: evidence from crossed-hands experiments , 2018, Psychological research.
[21] R. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[22] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.
[23] R. D. de Jong,et al. Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] R. Gottsdanker. A psychological refractory period or an unprepared period , 1979 .
[26] Michael E J Masson,et al. A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing , 2011, Behavior research methods.
[27] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Anticipatory affect during action preparation: evidence from backward compatibility in dual-task performance , 2017, Cognition & emotion.
[28] Moritz Durst,et al. A comparison of the psychological refractory period and prioritized processing paradigms: Can the response-selection bottleneck model explain them both? , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Gernot Horstmann,et al. The psychological refractory period of stopping. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[33] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .
[34] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance , 2014, Cognition.
[35] Lynn Huestegge,et al. Effects of a no-go Task 2 on Task 1 performance in dual - tasking: From benefits to costs , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[37] Erwin Hennighausen,et al. N200 in the Eriksen-Task: Inhibitory Executive Processes? , 2000 .
[38] Jeff Miller,et al. Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies , 2006, Psychological research.
[39] A. Miyake,et al. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[40] G. Logan,et al. Parallel response selection in dual-task situations , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] Paul Bertelson,et al. Refractory period of c-reactions , 1969 .
[42] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.
[43] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[44] R avid Ell. Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect , 2008 .
[45] A. Nambu,et al. No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects , 1993, Neuroscience Research.
[46] Moritz Durst,et al. “Just do it when you get a chance”: the effects of a background task on primary task performance , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[47] Victor Mittelstädt,et al. Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] R. Ratcliff,et al. A model of the go/no-go task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[49] Markus Janczyk,et al. A Common Capacity Limitation for Response and Item Selection in Working Memory , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[50] Markus Janczyk,et al. Confidence intervals for two sample means: Calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules , 2013, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[51] E. Wagenmakers. A practical solution to the pervasive problems ofp values , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[52] Eva Röttger,et al. Investigating the characteristics of “not responding”: backward crosstalk in the PRP paradigm with forced vs. free no-go decisions , 2017, Psychological research.
[53] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[54] Markus Janczyk,et al. Identifying the Locus of Compatibility-Based Backward Crosstalk: Evidence From an Extended PRP Paradigm , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] B. Kerr,et al. Preplanning for aimed movements: Disruption from a preliminary task. , 1983 .
[56] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Exceptions to the PRP Effect ? A Comparison of Prepared and Unconditioned Reflexes , 2014 .
[57] Rolf Ulrich,et al. On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[58] Jeff Miller,et al. Locus of backward crosstalk effects on task 1 in a psychological refractory period task. , 2014, Experimental psychology.
[59] Torsten Schubert,et al. Effects of extensive dual-task practice on processing stages in simultaneous choice tasks , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.