The Motor Locus of No-Go Backward Crosstalk

A frequent observation in dual-task studies is the backward crosstalk effect (BCE), meaning that aspects of a secondary Task 2 influence Task 1 performance. Up to this point, 2 major types of the BCE were investigated: a BCE based on dimensional overlap between both stimuli and/or responses (the compatibility-based BCE), and a BCE based on whether Task 2 is a go or no-go task (the no-go BCE). Recent evidence suggests that the compatibility-based BCE has its locus inside the response selection stage. The available evidence for the locus of the no-go BCE is still mixed, however. To this end, the 3 experiments reported in the present study used an extended psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm with 3 subsequent tasks. Applying the locus of slack logic in Experiment 1, the no-go BCE was not absorbed into the cognitive slack and, thus, a locus before response selection could be ruled out. Subsequently applying the effect propagation logic in Experiment 2 and 3, the no-go BCE arising in Task 1 was even inverted in Task 3. Because no propagation of the no-go BCE was observed, a locus before or in response selection could be ruled out. Thus, we conclude that the no-go BCE has its locus during motor execution. Because the no-go BCE and the compatibility-based BCE are located in different stages, we suggest that both types of the BCE do not share a common underlying mechanism.

[1]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  M. C. Smith,et al.  The Psychological Refractory Period as a Function of Performance of a First Response , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Ann Reynolds,et al.  The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Richard Schweickert,et al.  A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .

[5]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[6]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Dissociating interference-control processes between memory and response. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  R W Proctor,et al.  Multiple spatial correspondence effects on dual-task performance. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  C. Brunia Motor preparation: changes in amplitude of Achilles tendon reflexes during a fixed foreperiod of one second. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[9]  A. Welford THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .

[10]  F. Donders On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[11]  Scott Watter Parallel Response Selection in Dual -Tasks , 2003 .

[12]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Psychophysiological Measurement of Backward Response Activation in the Prioritized Processing Paradigm , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  C. W. Telford The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .

[15]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  Sequential modulation of backward crosstalk and task-shielding in dual-tasking. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Scott Watter,et al.  PRP training shows Task1 response selection is the locus of the backward response compatibility effect , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Motor limitation in dual-task processing with different effectors , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  The locus of tool-transformation costs. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  Action effect features, but not anatomical features, determine the Backward Crosstalk Effect: evidence from crossed-hands experiments , 2018, Psychological research.

[21]  R. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Automatic and controlled stimulus processing in conflict tasks: Superimposed diffusion processes and delta functions , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  R. D. de Jong,et al.  Multiple bottlenecks in overlapping task performance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R. Gottsdanker A psychological refractory period or an unprepared period , 1979 .

[26]  Michael E J Masson,et al.  A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[27]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Anticipatory affect during action preparation: evidence from backward compatibility in dual-task performance , 2017, Cognition & emotion.

[28]  Moritz Durst,et al.  A comparison of the psychological refractory period and prioritized processing paradigms: Can the response-selection bottleneck model explain them both? , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  B. Hommel Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Gernot Horstmann,et al.  The psychological refractory period of stopping. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[34]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance , 2014, Cognition.

[35]  Lynn Huestegge,et al.  Effects of a no-go Task 2 on Task 1 performance in dual - tasking: From benefits to costs , 2017, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Erwin Hennighausen,et al.  N200 in the Eriksen-Task: Inhibitory Executive Processes? , 2000 .

[38]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies , 2006, Psychological research.

[39]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  G. Logan,et al.  Parallel response selection in dual-task situations , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  Refractory period of c-reactions , 1969 .

[42]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Control of stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance , 2002, Psychological research.

[43]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[44]  R avid Ell Working memory involvement in dual-task performance: Evidence from the backward compatibility effect , 2008 .

[45]  A. Nambu,et al.  No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects , 1993, Neuroscience Research.

[46]  Moritz Durst,et al.  “Just do it when you get a chance”: the effects of a background task on primary task performance , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[47]  Victor Mittelstädt,et al.  Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A model of the go/no-go task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  A Common Capacity Limitation for Response and Item Selection in Working Memory , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  Confidence intervals for two sample means: Calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules , 2013, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[51]  E. Wagenmakers A practical solution to the pervasive problems ofp values , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  Eva Röttger,et al.  Investigating the characteristics of “not responding”: backward crosstalk in the PRP paradigm with forced vs. free no-go decisions , 2017, Psychological research.

[53]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[54]  Markus Janczyk,et al.  Identifying the Locus of Compatibility-Based Backward Crosstalk: Evidence From an Extended PRP Paradigm , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  B. Kerr,et al.  Preplanning for aimed movements: Disruption from a preliminary task. , 1983 .

[56]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Exceptions to the PRP Effect ? A Comparison of Prepared and Unconditioned Reflexes , 2014 .

[57]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[58]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Locus of backward crosstalk effects on task 1 in a psychological refractory period task. , 2014, Experimental psychology.

[59]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Effects of extensive dual-task practice on processing stages in simultaneous choice tasks , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.