Contributions of vision–proprioception interactions to the estimation of time-varying hand and target locations

We investigated the relative importance of vision and proprioception in estimating target and hand locations in a dynamic environment. Subjects performed a position estimation task in which a target moved horizontally on a screen at a constant velocity and then disappeared. They were asked to estimate the position of the invisible target under two conditions: passively observing and manually tracking. The tracking trials included three visual conditions with a cursor representing the hand position: always visible, disappearing simultaneously with target disappearance, and always invisible. The target’s invisible displacement was systematically underestimated during passive observation. In active conditions, tracking with the visible cursor significantly decreased the extent of underestimation. Tracking of the invisible target became much more accurate under this condition and was not affected by cursor disappearance. In a second experiment, subjects were asked to judge the position of their unseen hand instead of the target during tracking movements. Invisible hand displacements were also underestimated when compared with the actual displacement. Continuous or brief presentation of the cursor reduced the extent of underestimation. These results suggest that vision–proprioception interactions are critical for representing exact target–hand spatial relationships, and that such sensorimotor representation of hand kinematics serves a cognitive function in predicting target position. We propose a hypothesis that the central nervous system can utilize information derived from proprioception and/or efference copy for sensorimotor prediction of dynamic target and hand positions, but that effective use of this information for conscious estimation requires that it be presented in a form that corresponds to that used for the estimations.

[1]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  U. Proske,et al.  Digital Object Identifier (DOI) 10.1007/s002210000380 RESEARCH ARTICLE , 2022 .

[3]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Marion R. Van Horn,et al.  Tracking an invisible target reveals spatial tuning of neurons in the rostral superior colliculus is not dependent on visual stimuli. , 2009 .

[5]  Liang-Shih Fan,et al.  Electrical capacitance tomography imaging of gas-solid and gas-liquid-solid fluidized bed systems , 2004, J. Vis..

[6]  D. Perrett,et al.  Neuronal representation of disappearing and hidden objects in temporal cortex of the macaque , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Representation of hand position prior to movement and motor variability. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[8]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered and body-centered frames of reference in direct visuomotor transformations , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Prediction in the oculomotor system: smooth pursuit during transient disappearance of a visual target , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  H. Ross,et al.  Optic-Flow and Cognitive Factors in Time-to-Collision Estimates , 1983, Perception.

[11]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[12]  Robert J. van Beers,et al.  How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Role of sensory information in updating internal models of the effector during arm tracking. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[14]  G. Barnes,et al.  Human ocular pursuit during the transient disappearance of a visual target. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  D Regan,et al.  Visual factors in hitting and catching. , 1997, Journal of sports sciences.

[16]  Graham R Barnes,et al.  Sequence learning in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye movements in humans. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[17]  J. L. Gordon,et al.  A model of the smooth pursuit eye movement system , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[18]  Walter Levitsky,et al.  Eye Tracking of Observer-Generated Target Movements , 2005 .

[19]  M. Wexler,et al.  Movement prediction and movement production. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  S Jordan,et al.  Ocular pursuit movement as a function of visual and proprioceptive stimulation. , 1970, Vision research.

[22]  Richard Apps,et al.  An internal model of a moving visual target in the lateral cerebellum , 2009, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  P. Viviani,et al.  Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Neuronal correlates of inferred motion in primate posterior parietal cortex , 1995, Nature.

[25]  R Gray,et al.  Exploring the Link between Time to Collision and Representational Momentum , 2001, Perception.

[26]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  L. Mitrani,et al.  Pursuit eye movements of a disappearing moving target , 1978, Vision Research.

[28]  D. Lyon,et al.  Time course of visual extrapolation accuracy. , 1995, Acta psychologica.

[29]  G. Gauthier,et al.  Oculo-manual tracking of visual targets in monkey: role of the arm afferent information in the control of arm and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  K. Nagai,et al.  SCN output drives the autonomic nervous system: with special reference to the autonomic function related to the regulation of glucose metabolism. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[32]  G. M. Gauthier,et al.  Eye tracking of self-moved targets in the absence of vision , 1976, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  G. Barnes,et al.  The mechanism of prediction in human smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[34]  C. J. S. Collins,et al.  Scaling of smooth anticipatory eye velocity in response to sequences of discrete target movements in humans , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  D. Perrett,et al.  Perceptual History Influences Neural Responses to Face and Body Postures , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  D. Kahneman Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[38]  W Schiff,et al.  Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.

[39]  P R DeLucia,et al.  Cognitive motion extrapolation and cognitive clocking in prediction motion task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[41]  D. Rosenbaum Perception and extrapolation of velocity and acceleration. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  G. M. Gauthier,et al.  Oculo-manual tracking of visual targets: control learning, coordination control and coordination model , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  R. Miall,et al.  Adaptation to visual feedback delays in manual tracking: evidence against the Smith Predictor model of human visually guided action , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  R. Miall,et al.  Effects of visual feedback on manual tracking and action tremor in Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  D. Washburn,et al.  Can monkeys (Macaca mulatta) represent invisible displacement? , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[49]  Shraga Hocherman,et al.  Proprioceptive guidance and motor planning of reaching movements to unseen targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  V Cavallo,et al.  Visual Information and Skill Level in Time-To-Collision Estimation , 1988, Perception.

[51]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  R. Miall,et al.  On-line feedback control of human visually guided slow ramp tracking: effects of spatial separation of visual cues , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.