The expectancies that govern the P300 amplitude are mostly automatic and unconscious

We argue that probability effects on P300 amplitude are the product of an automatic frequency detector not subject to voluntary control and relatively inaccessible to consciousness. “Expectancies” related to P300 therefore appear to be passive, perceptual ones. If probability-based expectancies do become conscious, they are inversely related to P300, supporting the view of Donchin & Coles (1988).

[1]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Language-Related ERPs: Scalp Distributions and Modulation by Word Type and Semantic Priming , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  R. Knight,et al.  Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3 , 1989, Brain Research.

[3]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  J Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. I. Simple and choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[5]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Ford,et al.  The relationship between P300 amplitude and regional gray matter volumes depends upon the attentional system engaged. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  W. Sommer,et al.  Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  E. Halgren,et al.  Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. I. Superior temporal plane and parietal lobe. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[9]  E. Halgren,et al.  Intracerebral potentials to rare target and distractor auditory and visual stimuli. II. Medial, lateral and posterior temporal lobe. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  C C Wood,et al.  Event-related potentials elicited by deviant endings to melodies. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[12]  R. Verleger Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  Cyma Van Petten,et al.  Words and sentences: event-related brain potential measures. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[14]  R Verleger,et al.  Suspense and surprise: on the relationship between expectancies and P3. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[15]  S. Yamaguchi,et al.  Anterior and posterior association cortex contributions to the somatosensory P300 , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  D. Swinney,et al.  Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  A D Friederici,et al.  Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[19]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[20]  D. Potter,et al.  Normal P 300 following extensive damage to the left medial temporal lobe , 2022 .

[21]  D. Potter,et al.  Visual and auditory P300 in a case of bilateral destruction of the medial temporal lobes following viral encephalitis , 1993 .

[22]  Wolfgang Klimesch,et al.  Memory processes described as brain oscillations in the EEG-alpha and theta bands. , 1995 .

[23]  H. Neville,et al.  Maturational Constraints on Functional Specializations for Language Processing: ERP and Behavioral Evidence in Bilingual Speakers , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  F. Rösler The P300 event-related potentials: A one-humped dromedary's saddle on a two-humped camel , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  M. Onofrj,et al.  P3 recordings in patients with bilateral temporal lobe lesions , 1992, Neurology.

[26]  R. Johnson,et al.  Auditory and visual P300s in temporal lobectomy patients: evidence for modality-dependent generators. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[27]  A. Tversky,et al.  Variants of uncertainty , 1982, Cognition.

[28]  L. Squire,et al.  P300 from amnesic patients with bilateral hippocampal lesions. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  D. Potter,et al.  Normal P300 following extensive damage to the left medial temporal lobe. , 1991, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[30]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  R. Verleger On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[32]  F. Macar,et al.  An event-related potential analysis of incongruity in music and other non-linguistic contexts. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[33]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[34]  R Verleger,et al.  P3-evoking wrong notes: unexpected, awaited, or arousing? , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[35]  W. Sommer,et al.  Differential effects of voluntary expectancies on reaction times and event-related potentials: evidence for automatic and controlled expectancies. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Late ERP components in visual and auditory Go/Nogo tasks. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  E Donchin,et al.  Sequential expectancies and decision making in a changing environment: an electrophysiological approach. , 1982, Psychophysiology.

[38]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Late visual and auditory ERP components and choice reaction time , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[39]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[40]  C. Van Petten,et al.  Words and sentences: event-related brain potential measures. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[41]  E Donchin,et al.  "...'Twas ten to one; and yet we ventured...": P300 and decision making. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[42]  O. Neumann Visual Attention and Action , 1990 .

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[44]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of choice complexity on different subcomponents of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[45]  C. Blakemore The Working Brain , 1972, Nature.

[46]  Emanuel Donchin,et al.  The varieties of deviant experience: ERP manifestations of deviance processors , 1997 .

[47]  Walter Ritter,et al.  The relation of P3b to prior events and future behavior , 1984, Biological Psychology.

[48]  R. Verleger,et al.  Reduction of P3b in patients with temporo-parietal lesions. , 1994, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.