The influence of predation risk on the feeding motivation and foraging strategy of juvenile Atlantic salmon

Juvenile Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, changed their foraging strategy markedly after a brief exposure to a model trout predator, in ways that reduced their conspicuousness and hence risk of being preyed upon. After seeing the predator, the salmon were less likely to orientate to passing food particles, and having orientated, were less likely to attack them. They also reduced the extent of their movements, by only attacking those food particles that came close to them, and by delaying attacks until the food had reached its closest point. They were slower to orientate to approaching food items, possibly because more visual attention was switched to scanning for predators and less to feeding. However, there was no change in the outcome of attacks, indicating that the reduced food intake was not caused by a reduction in appetite. The relative priority given to foraging and predator avoidance varied with time elapsed since the predator was last sighted; as a consequence, intake rates in the 20 min following the predator presentation averaged only 33% of the pre-predator level, but had increased to 57% 20 min later, and had recovered completely within 2 h.

[1]  Thomas Caraco,et al.  Avian flocking in the presence of a predator , 1980, Nature.

[2]  P. Colgan Motivational Analysis of Fish Feeding , 1973 .

[3]  E. S. Lawrence Vigilance during ‘easy’ and ‘difficult’ foraging tasks , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  D. J. Hall,et al.  An Experimental Test of the Effects of Predation Risk on Habitat Use in Fish , 1983 .

[5]  J. Thorpe,et al.  The role of food particle size in the growth of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.) , 1979 .

[6]  C. Barnard,et al.  Flock feeding and time budgets in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus L.) , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  M. Milinski A predator's costs of overcoming the confusion-effect of swarming prey , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  R. A. Bobbitt,et al.  Sex differences in the development of independence of infant monkeys. , 1968, Behaviour.

[9]  A. Shanks,et al.  Effects of pre‐ and post‐prandial starvation on meal size and evacuation rate of juvenile Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar L. , 1984 .

[10]  Neil B. Metcalfe,et al.  The effects of habitat on the vigilance of shorebirds: Is visibility important? , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  Andrew Sih,et al.  Foraging Strategies and the Avoidance of Predation by an Aquatic Insect, Notonecta Hoffmanni , 1982 .

[12]  W. A. Donnelly,et al.  Evidence for crypsis in coho salmon, Oncorhynchus kisutch (Walbaum), parr: substrate colour preference and achromatic reflectance , 1984 .

[13]  Anne E. Magurran,et al.  Predator model recognition and response habituation in shoaling minnows , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[14]  M. Milinski,et al.  Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.) , 1978, Nature.

[15]  F. Huntingford,et al.  Seasonal changes in feeding motivation of juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) , 1986 .

[16]  S. L. Lima,et al.  Foraging-efficiency-predation-risk trade-off in the grey squirrel , 1985, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  D. Fraser,et al.  Predation and Risk in Foraging Minnows: Balancing Conflicting Demands , 1983, The American Naturalist.

[18]  J. Beukema Predation by the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.): the influence of hunger and experience. , 1968, Behaviour.

[19]  R. Sibly How incentive and deficit determine feeding tendency , 1975, Animal Behaviour.