Judging roughness by sight—A 7‐tesla fMRI study on responsivity of the primary somatosensory cortex during observed touch of self and others

Observing another person being touched activates our own somatosensory system. Whether the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) is also activated during the observation of passive touch, and which subregions of S1 are responsible for self‐ and other‐related observed touch is currently unclear. In our study, we first aimed to clarify whether observing passive touch without any action component can robustly increase activity in S1. Secondly, we investigated whether S1 activity only increases when touch of others is observed, or also when touch of one's own body is observed. We were particularly interested in which subregions of S1 are responsible for either process. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging at 7 Tesla to measure S1 activity changes when participants observed videos of their own or another's hand in either egocentric or allocentric perspective being touched by different pieces of sandpaper. Participants were required to judge the roughness of the different sandpaper surfaces. Our results clearly show that S1 activity does increase in response to observing passive touch, and that activity changes are localized in posterior but not in anterior parts of S1. Importantly, activity increases in S1 were particularly related to observing another person being touched. Self‐related observed touch, in contrast, caused no significant activity changes within S1. We therefore assume that posterior but not anterior S1 is part of a system for sharing tactile experiences with others. Hum Brain Mapp, 2013. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[2]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  Paul T. Sowden,et al.  Your hand or mine? The extrastriate body area , 2008, NeuroImage.

[4]  W. Singer,et al.  Cortical responses to self and others , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[5]  M. Heller Visual and tactual texture perception: Intersensory cooperation , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  P. Bandettini,et al.  What's New in Neuroimaging Methods? , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  My body or yours? The effect of visual perspective on cortical body representations. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  Gaspare Galati,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Juliana Dushanova,et al.  Neurons in primary motor cortex engaged during action observation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  Robin M Heidemann,et al.  Generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA) , 2002, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[11]  S-J Blakemore,et al.  Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  R. Hari,et al.  Modulated Activation of the Human SI and SII Cortices during Observation of Hand Actions , 2002, NeuroImage.

[13]  K. Zilles,et al.  Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex 2. Spatial Normalization to Standard Anatomical Space , 2000, NeuroImage.

[14]  Tania Singer,et al.  The painful side of empathy , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Joaquín M. Fuster,et al.  Neuronal activity of somatosensory cortex in a cross-modal (visuo-haptic) memory task , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Here I am: The cortical correlates of visual self-recognition , 2007, Brain Research.

[18]  J. Decety,et al.  The functional architecture of human empathy. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[19]  S. Francis,et al.  Mapping human somatosensory cortex in individual subjects with 7 T functional MRI 1 Running title : Mapping human somatosensory cortex , 2010 .

[20]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Tobias Kober,et al.  MP2RAGE, a self bias-field corrected sequence for improved segmentation and T1-mapping at high field , 2010, NeuroImage.

[22]  Michael Schaefer,et al.  Effects of different viewing perspectives on somatosensory activations during observation of touch , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[23]  Amanda L. Kaas,et al.  Distinct cortical networks for the detection and identification of human body , 2009, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy , 2006, Social Neuroscience.

[25]  A. Schleicher,et al.  Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex 1. Microstructural Organization and Interindividual Variability , 1999, NeuroImage.

[26]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Vision of the body modulates processing in primary somatosensory cortex , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  H. Alkadhi,et al.  Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus. A new landmark. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Somatosensation in social perception , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Kaas,et al.  What, if anything, is SI? Organization of first somatosensory area of cortex. , 1983, Physiological reviews.

[30]  J. Ward,et al.  Mirror-touch synesthesia is linked with empathy , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Seung-Schik Yoo,et al.  Neural substrates of tactile imagery: a functional MRI study , 2003, Neuroreport.

[32]  Amishi P. Jha,et al.  Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain , 2005, NeuroImage.

[33]  C. Keysers,et al.  The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Allodynia: a sensory analogue of motor mirror neurons in a hyperaesthetic patient reporting instantaneous discomfort to another's perceived sudden minor injury? , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  L. Fogassi,et al.  A Touching Sight SII/PV Activation during the Observation and Experience of Touch , 2004, Neuron.

[36]  L. White,et al.  Structure of the human sensorimotor system. I: Morphology and cytoarchitecture of the central sulcus. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  P. Haggard,et al.  The rubber hand illusion: Sensitivity and reference frame for body ownership , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[38]  G. Pellegrino,et al.  Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: Is empathy a multisensory issue? , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  Akram Aldroubi,et al.  B-spline signal processing. II. Efficiency design and applications , 1993, IEEE Trans. Signal Process..

[40]  K. Zilles,et al.  Human Somatosensory Area 2: Observer-Independent Cytoarchitectonic Mapping, Interindividual Variability, and Population Map , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Observing touch activates human primary somatosensory cortex , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[43]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Don't Do It! Cortical Inhibition and Self-attribution during Action Observation , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  P. Downing,et al.  The effect of viewpoint on body representation in the extrastriate body area , 2004, Neuroreport.

[45]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Testing anatomically specified hypotheses in functional imaging using cytoarchitectonic maps , 2006, NeuroImage.

[46]  Michael Unser,et al.  B-spline signal processing. I. Theory , 1993, IEEE Trans. Signal Process..

[47]  A M Dale,et al.  Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  Jonathan D. Nelson,et al.  Human cortical representations for reaching: Mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery , 2007, NeuroImage.

[49]  K. Uğurbil,et al.  Spin‐echo fMRI in humans using high spatial resolutions and high magnetic fields , 2003, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[50]  Vittorio Gallese,et al.  The Sense of Touch: Embodied Simulation in a Visuotactile Mirroring Mechanism for Observed Animate or Inanimate Touch , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  K. Uğurbil,et al.  Experimental determination of the BOLD field strength dependence in vessels and tissue , 1997, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[52]  Peter G. Morris,et al.  fMRI at 1.5, 3 and 7 T: Characterising BOLD signal changes , 2009, NeuroImage.

[53]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition , 2002, Cognition.

[54]  P. Haggard,et al.  Self and Other in the Human Motor System , 2006, Current Biology.

[55]  J. Driver,et al.  Multisensory Interplay Reveals Crossmodal Influences on ‘Sensory-Specific’ Brain Regions, Neural Responses, and Judgments , 2008, Neuron.

[56]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[58]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Stimulus-driven modulation of motor-evoked potentials during observation of others' pain , 2006, NeuroImage.

[59]  J. Régis,et al.  Three-dimensional reconstruction of the human central sulcus reveals a morphological correlate of the hand area. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Unified segmentation , 2005, NeuroImage.

[61]  Mariagrazia Benassi,et al.  Selective impairment of self body-parts processing in right brain-damaged patients , 2010, Cortex.

[62]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[63]  Hiroaki Ishida,et al.  Shared Mapping of Own and Others' Bodies in Visuotactile Bimodal Area of Monkey Parietal Cortex , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[65]  A M Dale,et al.  Segregation of somatosensory activation in the human rolandic cortex using fMRI. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  Emanuela Galante,et al.  Is it Mine? Hemispheric Asymmetries in Corporeal Self-recognition , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Is Empathy-Induced Helping Due to Self–Other Merging? , 1997 .

[68]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  Michael Erb,et al.  Visual features of an observed agent do not modulate human brain activity during action observation , 2009, NeuroImage.

[70]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited , 2007, NeuroImage.

[71]  John L. Bradshaw,et al.  Shared pain: From empathy to synaesthesia , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[72]  John C. Gore,et al.  Differentiation of somatosensory cortices by high-resolution fMRI at 7T , 2011, NeuroImage.

[73]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Actor's and observer's primary motor cortices stabilize similarly after seen or heard motor actions , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[74]  Xenophon Papademetris,et al.  Spatial resolution, signal-to-noise ratio, and smoothing in multi-subject functional MRI studies , 2006, NeuroImage.

[75]  Xiaosi Gu,et al.  Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[76]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy , 2005, NeuroImage.