Instructional Efficiency: Revisiting the Original Construct in Educational Research

This article revisits Paas and Van Merriënboer's (1993) measure of instructional efficiency, which can be applied by educational researchers to compare the effects of different instructional conditions on learning. This measure relied on performance and mental effort on the test, and as such gave an indication of the quality of learning outcomes. The acquisition of more (less) efficient cognitive schemata is indicated by combinations of high (low) performance and low (high) mental effort. This instructional efficiency measure has become widely adopted, but in an adapted form that incorporates mental effort invested in the learning phase instead of the test phase. This article demonstrates that the adaptation has important consequences for the construct of instructional efficiency and for the type of conclusions that can be drawn. Examples are given to illustrate the various implications of different combinations of mental effort and performance measures in the light of more contemporary developments in educational research.

[1]  M. D. Vernon Learning and Understanding , 1951 .

[2]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[3]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[4]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load During Problem Solving: Effects on Learning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  A. Bandura Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. , 1989 .

[6]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[7]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Students' motivational beliefs and their cognitive engagement in classroom academic tasks. , 1992 .

[8]  Fred G. W. C. Paas,et al.  The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures , 1992 .

[9]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[10]  R. Carey,et al.  Understanding instructions. , 1992, Journal of obstetric, gynecologic, and neonatal nursing : JOGNN.

[11]  Katherine S. Cennamo,et al.  Learning from video: Factors influencing learners' preconceptions and invested mental effort , 1993 .

[12]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[13]  J. Anderson,et al.  Acquisition of procedural skills from examples. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  F. Paas,et al.  Measurement of Cognitive Load in Instructional Research , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[15]  K. Teigen Yerkes-Dodson: A Law for all Seasons , 1994 .

[16]  J. Sweller,et al.  Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes , 1995 .

[17]  Paul Chandler,et al.  Some Conditions under Which Integrated Computer-Based Training Software Can Facilitate Learning , 1996 .

[18]  Sharon K Tindall-Ford,et al.  When two sensory modes are better than one , 1997 .

[19]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive load theory and instructional design , 1997 .

[20]  Yeung,et al.  Cognitive Load and Learner Expertise: Split-Attention and Redundancy Effects in Reading with Explanatory Notes , 1998, Contemporary educational psychology.

[21]  Paul Chandler,et al.  Levels of Expertise and Instructional Design , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[22]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[23]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction , 1999 .

[24]  D. Gopher,et al.  Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application. , 1999 .

[25]  J. Tuovinen,et al.  A comparison of cognitive load associated with discovery learning and worked examples , 1999 .

[26]  A. Yeung Cognitive Load and Learner Expertise: Split-Attention and Redundancy Effects in Reading Comprehension Tasks With Vocabulary Definitions , 1999 .

[27]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Assessing our own competence: Heuristics and illusions. , 1999 .

[28]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Incorporating Learner Experience into the Design of Multimedia Instruction. , 2000 .

[29]  Fred Paas,et al.  Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: a comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency , 2001, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[30]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Learner Experience and Efficiency of Instructional Guidance , 2001 .

[31]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[32]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. , 2001 .

[33]  Sharon K Tindall-Ford,et al.  Learning by imagining. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[34]  Mark S. Young,et al.  Malleable Attentional Resources Theory: A New Explanation for the Effects of Mental Underload on Performance , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[35]  J. G. Schuurman,et al.  Redirecting learners' attention during training: Effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training efficiency. , 2002 .

[36]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive load theory and aging: effects of worked examples on training efficiency , 2002 .

[37]  Stephen M. Fiore,et al.  Scaffolding cognitive and metacognitive processes in low verbal ability learners: Use of diagrams in computer-based training environments , 2002 .

[38]  P. Chandler,et al.  Assimilating complex information , 2002 .

[39]  R. Atkinson,et al.  Transitioning From Studying Examples to Solving Problems: Effects of Self-Explanation Prompts and Fading Worked-Out Steps. , 2003 .

[40]  F. Paas,et al.  The efficiency of multimedia learning into old age. , 2003, The British journal of educational psychology.

[41]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  The Expertise Reversal Effect , 2003 .

[42]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory , 2003 .

[43]  D. Leutner,et al.  Direct Measurement of Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[44]  P. Chandler,et al.  Learning and understanding science instructional material. , 2003 .

[45]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments , 2003 .

[46]  Fred Paas,et al.  Cueing for transfer in multimedia programmes: process worksheets vs. worked-out examples , 2004, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[47]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures , 2004 .

[48]  Fred Paas,et al.  Mental Effort and Performance as Determinants for the Dynamic Selection of Learning Tasks in Air Traffic Control Training , 2004 .

[49]  Paul Chandler,et al.  When Redundant On-Screen Text in Multimedia Technical Instruction Can Interfere With Learning , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[50]  R. Moreno Decreasing Cognitive Load for Novice Students: Effects of Explanatory versus Corrective Feedback in Discovery-Based Multimedia , 2004 .

[51]  Fred Paas,et al.  Process-Oriented Worked Examples: Improving Transfer Performance Through Enhanced Understanding , 2004 .

[52]  Liesbeth Kester,et al.  Timing of Information Presentation in Learning Statistics , 2004 .

[53]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory: Instructional Implications of the Interaction between Information Structures and Cognitive Architecture , 2004 .

[54]  F. Paas,et al.  Memory load and the cognitive pupillary response in aging. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Gillian B. Yeo,et al.  A multilevel analysis of effort, practice, and performance: effects; of ability, conscientiousness, and goal orientation. , 2004, The Journal of applied psychology.

[56]  Fred Paas,et al.  Exploring Multidimensional Approaches to the Efficiency of Instructional Conditions , 2004 .

[57]  R. Moreno,et al.  Cognitive load and learning effects of having students organize pictures and words in multimedia environments: The role of student interactivity and feedback , 2005 .

[58]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning , 2005 .

[59]  Abdel K. Halabi,et al.  Empirical Evidence on the Relative Efficiency of Worked Examples versus Problem‐Solving Exercises in Accounting Principles Instruction , 2005 .

[60]  F. Paas,et al.  A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learner involvement in instruction , 2005 .

[61]  J. Sweller Implications of Cognitive Load Theory for Multimedia Learning , 2005, The Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning.

[62]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent Developments and Future Directions , 2005 .

[63]  T. Gog,et al.  Uncovering Expertise-Related Differences in Troubleshooting Performance: Combining Eye Movement and Concurrent Verbal Protocol Data , 2005 .

[64]  Paul Ayres Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems , 2006 .

[65]  Liesbeth Kester,et al.  Just-in-time, schematic supportive information presentation during cognitive skill acquisition , 2006, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[66]  T. Gog,et al.  Optimising worked example instruction: Different ways to increase germane cognitive load. , 2006 .

[67]  Bing Hiong Ngu,et al.  Evaluating a CALL software on the learning of English prepositions , 2006, Comput. Educ..

[68]  Paul A. Kirschner,et al.  Just-in-time information presentation: Improving learning a troubleshooting skill. , 2006 .

[69]  Liesbeth Kester,et al.  Towards a personalized task selection model with shared instructional control , 2006 .

[70]  Slava Kalyuga Assessment of learners' organised knowledge structures in adaptive learning environments , 2006 .

[71]  Fred Paas,et al.  Dynamic Task Selection in Flight Management System Training , 2006 .

[72]  Fred Paas,et al.  Personalised adaptive task selection in air traffic control: Effects on training efficiency and transfer , 2006 .

[73]  R. Catrambone,et al.  Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? * , 2006 .

[74]  Fred Paas,et al.  Effects of Process-Oriented Worked Examples on Troubleshooting Transfer Performance , 2006 .

[75]  Béatrice S. Hasler,et al.  Learner Control, Cognitive Load and Instructional Animation , 2007 .

[76]  Fred Paas,et al.  Instructional efficiency of animation: effects of interactivity through mental reconstruction of static key frames , 2007 .

[77]  Fred Paas,et al.  Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency , 2008 .

[78]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[79]  David F. Feldon Cognitive load theory and instructional design , 2009 .