Delay discounting predicts cigarette smoking in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement

RationaleHigher rates of delay discounting, or impulsive choice, may be related to relapse during abstinence reinforcement interventions for cigarette smoking, and a transdermal nicotine patch may attenuate delay discounting.ObjectiveThe objectives of this study are to assess the relation between delay discounting and smoking after nicotine deprivation in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement and the effects of a transdermal nicotine patch on discounting and smoking.Materials and methodsSmokers with no self-reported intention to quit were randomly assigned to an active (14 mg) or placebo patch group (n = 15 per group). In each of three sessions, after a 3-h deprivation period, participants completed a delay discounting task, mood, and craving measures and finally engaged in a laboratory model of abstinence reinforcement. Three abstinence reinforcement conditions were presented in counterbalanced order across the three sessions. During the control session, monetary consequences were delivered every 30 s regardless of smoking. During the low ($5.00 available) and high ($20.00 available) sessions, participants could earn a progressively increasing amount of money for each 30 s period of abstinence.ResultsThe low and high conditions significantly increased the latency to smoke relative to control and significantly decreased the amount of smoking. The nicotine patch decreased negative affect, but it did not significantly affect delay discounting or smoking. Individuals who smoked during the low and high conditions showed higher rates of discounting.ConclusionThe patch did not attenuate delay discounting or smoking after a period of deprivation, but contingencies for abstinence significantly decreased smoking. Higher rates of delay discounting were related to smoking in a model of abstinence reinforcement treatment.

[1]  Warren K Bickel,et al.  Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. , 2002, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[2]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Effects of short-term nicotine deprivation on decision-making: delay, uncertainty and effort discounting. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[3]  Jesse Dallery,et al.  An Internet-based abstinence reinforcement treatment for cigarette smoking. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[4]  M. Stitzer,et al.  Medication take-home as a reinforcer in a methadone maintenance program. , 1977, Addictive behaviors.

[5]  N. Heather,et al.  A conceptual framework for explaining drug addiction , 1998, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[6]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. , 1998, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[7]  H. de Wit,et al.  Diazepam impairs behavioral inhibition but not delay discounting or risk taking in healthy adults. , 2006, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[8]  G. Badger,et al.  Influence of the duration of abstinence on the relative reinforcing effects of cigarette smoking , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[9]  G. Badger,et al.  A meta-analysis of voucher-based reinforcement therapy for substance use disorders. , 2006, Addiction.

[10]  G. Madden,et al.  Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[11]  S. Higgins,et al.  A within-subject comparison of three different schedules of reinforcement of drug abstinence using cigarette smoking as an exemplar. , 2000, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[12]  E. Wiseman,et al.  Effectiveness of payment for reduced carbon monoxide levels and noncontingent payments on smoking behaviors in cocaine-abusing outpatients wearing nicotine or placebo patches. , 2005, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[13]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Reduction of abstinence-induced withdrawal and craving using high-dose nicotine replacement therapy , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[15]  C. Johanson,et al.  The influence of exchange delays on cigarette versus money choice: a laboratory analog of voucher-based reinforcement therapy. , 2000, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[16]  A. Morice,et al.  Breath carbon monoxide as an indication of smoking habit. , 2000, Chest.

[17]  B. Spring,et al.  Impulsivity and smoking relapse. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[18]  D. Jacobs,et al.  Cigarette Smoking, Alcohol Consumption, and Risk of ARDS , 2000 .

[19]  M. Jarvik,et al.  Nicotine Blood Levels and Subjective Craving for Cigarettes , 2000, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[20]  S S Smith,et al.  Predicting smoking cessation. Who will quit with and without the nicotine patch. , 1994, JAMA.

[21]  S. Higgins,et al.  Contingent monetary reinforcement of smoking reductions, with and without transdermal nicotine, in outpatients with schizophrenia. , 2002, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[22]  B. Reynolds The Experiential Discounting Task is sensitive to cigarette-smoking status and correlates with a measure of delay discounting , 2006, Behavioural pharmacology.

[23]  W. Bickel,et al.  Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  Gregory J. Madden,et al.  Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[25]  Carla H. Lagorio,et al.  Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: Steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards , 2005, Behavioural Processes.

[26]  Bethany R Raiff,et al.  Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Between- and within-subject comparisons. , 2004, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[27]  J. Henningfield,et al.  Effects of transdermal nicotine delivery on measures of acute nicotine withdrawal. , 1996, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[28]  H. Rachlin Four teleological theories of addiction , 1997 .

[29]  J. Hughes Tobacco withdrawal in self-quitters. , 1992, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[30]  A. Christopoulos,et al.  Fitting Models to Biological Data Using Linear and Nonlinear Regression: A Practical Guide to Curve Fitting , 2004 .

[31]  K. Silverman,et al.  Voucher-based reinforcement of cocaine abstinence in treatment-resistant methadone patients: effects of reinforcement magnitude , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  G. Madden,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards in opioid-dependent outpatients: exponential or hyperbolic discounting functions? , 1999, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[33]  J. Elster,et al.  Choice Over Time , 1992 .

[34]  G. Badger,et al.  Incentives improve outcome in outpatient behavioral treatment of cocaine dependence. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[35]  S. Shoptaw,et al.  A Comparison of Five Reinforcement Schedules for use in Contingency Management-Based Treatment of Methamphetamine Abuse , 2006 .

[36]  Taiki Takahashi,et al.  Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[37]  G. Badger,et al.  A pilot study on voucher-based incentives to promote abstinence from cigarette smoking during pregnancy and postpartum. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[38]  Andrew Goudie,et al.  Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: the effects of nicotine deprivation , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[39]  George Loewenstein,et al.  Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioid-dependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[40]  K. Schuh,et al.  Desire to smoke during spaced smoking intervals , 1995, Psychopharmacology.

[41]  L. Green,et al.  Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. , 2001, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[42]  S. Higgins,et al.  The validity of the reinstatement model of craving and relapse to drug use , 2003, Psychopharmacology.

[43]  T S Critchfield,et al.  Temporal discounting: basic research and the analysis of socially important behavior. , 2001, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[44]  C. Lerman,et al.  Rate of nicotine onset from nicotine replacement therapy and acute responses in smokers. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[45]  J. McNeil,et al.  Prospective study of factors predicting outcome of transdermal nicotine treatment in smoking cessation , 1994, BMJ.

[46]  A. Waters,et al.  Smoking topography: reliability and validity in dependent smokers. , 2003, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[47]  J. Dallery,et al.  Effects of an Internet-based voucher reinforcement program for smoking abstinence: a feasibility study. , 2005, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[48]  L. Kozlowski,et al.  The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. , 1991, British journal of addiction.

[49]  Karl Fagerstrom,et al.  Measuring nicotine dependence: A review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire , 1989, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[50]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[51]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[52]  J. Dallery,et al.  Voucher-based reinforcement of opiate plus cocaine abstinence in treatment-resistant methadone patients: effects of reinforcer magnitude. , 2001, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[53]  W K Bickel,et al.  Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. , 1997, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[54]  Drazen Prelec,et al.  A Theory of Addiction , 1992 .

[55]  J. Prochaska,et al.  Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: toward an integrative model of change. , 1983, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[56]  B. Rosner,et al.  Predictors of smoking relapse among self-quitters: a report from the Normative Aging Study. , 1992, Addictive behaviors.