Do sequence-space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills? Yes, but there are individual differences

People with sequence-space synaesthesia perceive sequences (e.g. numbers, months, letters) as spatially extended forms. Here, we ask whether sequence-space synaesthetes have advantages in visuo-spatial skills such as mental rotation. Previous studies addressing this question have produced mixed results with some showing mental rotation advantages (Simner et al. in Cortex 45:1246–1260, 2009; Brang et al. in Cogn Process, 2013), but one that did not (Rizza and Price in Cogn Process 13:299–303, 2012). We tested this hypothesis again with a new group of sequence-space synaesthetes, and we also tested a range of individual differences that might have caused this conflict across previous studies. Specifically, we tested: years of education, visual imagery ability, nature of forms (2D or 3D representation of sequences), number of forms (e.g. for months, days, numbers), and tendency to project sequences into external space versus the mind’s eye. We found yet again that synaesthetes had enhanced abilities in mental rotation compared to controls, but that one individual difference in synaesthetes (the ability to project forms into space) was especially linked to performance. We also found that synaesthetes self-reported higher visual imagery than controls (Price in Cortex 45:1229–1245, 2009; Mann et al. in Conscious Cognit 18:619–627, 2009; Rizza and Price 2012). Overall, our data support previous studies showing superior imagery reports (Price 2009) and mental rotation (Simner et al. 2009; Brang et al. 2013) in sequence-space synaesthetes, and we suggest that one previous failure to replicate (Rizza and Price 2012) might be explained by individual differences among synaesthetes recruited for testing.

[1]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Individual Differences in Spatial Mental Imagery , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  T. Vecchi,et al.  A Neurological Dissociation Between Preserved Visual and Impaired Spatial Processing in Mental Imagery , 1998, Cortex.

[3]  Michael A. Motes,et al.  Object-Spatial Imagery: A New Self-Report Imagery Questionnaire , 2006 .

[4]  Mark C. Price Spatial forms and mental imagery , 2009, Cortex.

[5]  N. Sagiv,et al.  What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? , 2006, Cognition.

[6]  D. Smilek,et al.  A different outlook on time: Visual and auditory month names elicit different mental vantage points for a time-space synaesthete , 2009, Cortex.

[7]  M. Hegarty,et al.  Types of visual–spatial representations and mathematical problem solving. , 1999 .

[8]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Time–space synaesthesia – A cognitive advantage? , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  What information is critical to elicit interference in number-form synaesthesia? , 2009, Cortex.

[10]  L. Cooper Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  Jennifer M. Shephard,et al.  Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale , 2017 .

[12]  J. Simner Why are there different types of synesthete? , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[13]  S. E. Ball,et al.  Reliance on Visual Imagery and its Relation to Mental Rotation , 1997, Perceptual and motor skills.

[14]  M. Price Synesthesia, Imagery, and Performance , 2013 .

[15]  C. Pernet,et al.  Low and high imagers activate networks differentially in mental rotation , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  I. M. Harris,et al.  Selective right parietal lobe activation during mental rotation: a parametric PET study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  David M. Eagleman,et al.  The objectification of overlearned sequences: A new view of spatial sequence synesthesia , 2009, Cortex.

[18]  H. Scholte,et al.  Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  Mark C. Price,et al.  Do sequence-space synaesthetes have better spatial imagery skills? Maybe not , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[20]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Number Forms in the Brain , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Varieties of grapheme-colour synaesthesia: A new theory of phenomenological and behavioural differences , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[22]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Temporal sequences, synesthetic mappings, and cultural biases: The geography of time , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  Seana Coulson,et al.  Enhanced mental rotation ability in time-space synesthesia , 2013, Cognitive Processing.

[24]  D. F. Marks,et al.  Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. , 1973, British journal of psychology.

[25]  Edward M. Hubbard,et al.  Oxford Handbook of Synesthesia , 2013 .

[26]  Marie S. Burrage,et al.  Two Forms of Spatial Imagery , 2009, Psychological science.

[27]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Not all synaesthetes are created equal: Projector versus associator synaesthetes , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Visual mental imagery and visual perception: Structural equivalence revealed by scanning processes , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Jean-Baptiste Cazier,et al.  A whole-genome scan and fine-mapping linkage study of auditory-visual synesthesia reveals evidence of linkage to chromosomes 2q24, 5q33, 6p12, and 12p12. , 2009, American journal of human genetics.

[30]  Edward M. Hubbard,et al.  Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia , 2005, Neuron.

[31]  D. Levine,et al.  Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[33]  J. Simner,et al.  A foundation for savantism? Visuo-spatial synaesthetes present with cognitive benefits , 2009, Cortex.