Perceptual asynchrony for motion

Psychophysical experiments show that two different visual attributes, color and motion, processed in different areas of the visual brain, are perceived at different times relative to each other (Moutoussis and Zeki, 1997a). Here we demonstrate psychophysically that two variants of the same attribute, motion, which have the same temporal structure and are processed in the same visual areas, are also processed asynchronously. When subjects were asked to pair up–down motion of dots in one half of their hemifield with up-right motion in the other, they perceived the two directions of motion asynchronously, with the advantage in favor of up-right motion; when they were asked to pair the motion of white dots moving against a black background with that of red dots moving against an equiluminant green background, they perceived the luminant motion first, thus demonstrating a perceptual advantage of luminant over equiluminant motion. These results were not affected by motion speed or perceived motion “streaks.” We thus interpret these results to reflect the different processing times produced by luminant and equiluminant motion stimuli or by different degrees of motion direction change, thus adding to the evidence that processing time within the visual system is a major determinant of perceptual time.

[1]  B. B. Lee,et al.  Responses of macaque ganglion cells to movement of chromatic borders. , 1992, Journal of Physiology.

[2]  P. Viviani,et al.  Colour, form, and movement are not perceived simultaneously , 2001, Vision Research.

[3]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Constraints on the source of short-term motion adaptation in macaque area MT. I. the role of input and intrinsic mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Independent, synchronous access to color and motion features , 2008, Cognition.

[5]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Teller,et al.  Motion at isoluminance: motion dead zones in three-dimensional color space. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  D. Dennett,et al.  The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .

[8]  V. S. RAMACHANDRAN,et al.  Does colour provide an input to human motion perception? , 1978, Nature.

[9]  B. B. Lee,et al.  The physiological basis of heterochromatic flicker photometry demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the macaque retina. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  A. Holcombe Temporal binding favours the early phase of colour changes, but not of motion changes, yielding the colour–motion asynchrony illusion , 2009 .

[11]  J L Barbur,et al.  Visual processing levels revealed by response latencies to changes in different visual attributes , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  K. Moutoussis Asynchrony in Visual Consciousness and the Possible Involvement of Attention , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[13]  A. Mizuno,et al.  A change of the leading player in flow Visualization technique , 2006, J. Vis..

[14]  S. Zeki,et al.  The temporal order of binding visual attributes , 2006, Vision Research.

[15]  S. Zeki,et al.  The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  S. Nishida,et al.  Two mechanisms underlying the effect of angle of motion direction change on colour–motion asynchrony , 2007, Vision Research.

[17]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  Time and the Observer , 1992 .

[18]  K. T. Mullen,et al.  Absence of smooth motion perception in color vision , 1992, Vision Research.

[19]  Saumil S. Patel,et al.  Color and motion: which is the tortoise and which is the hare? , 2003, Vision Research.

[20]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Constraints on the source of short-term motion adaptation in macaque area MT. II. tuning of neural circuit mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  K. Moutoussis,et al.  Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  S. Nishida,et al.  Marker Correspondence, Not Processing Latency, Determines Temporal Binding of Visual Attributes , 2002, Current Biology.

[23]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Directionally selective response of cells in the middle temporal area (MT) of the macaque monkey to the movement of equiluminous opponent color stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  David Alais,et al.  Motion streaks in fast motion rivalry cause orientation-selective suppression. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[26]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Zeki,et al.  A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Perceptual pairing of colour and motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[29]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey. , 2001, Visual neuroscience.

[32]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.

[33]  G. O'Brien,et al.  The disunity of consciousness , 1998 .

[34]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Parallel and Serial Neural Mechanisms for Visual Search in Macaque Area V4 , 2005, Science.

[35]  W. A. Escobar,et al.  Quantized visual awareness , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[36]  S. Zeki,et al.  The asynchrony of consciousness , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[38]  B. B. Lee,et al.  The physiological basis of the minimally distinct border demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the macaque retina. , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[40]  Semir Zeki,et al.  The primary visual cortex, and feedback to it, are not necessary for conscious vision. , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  H. Ives XC. Studies in the photometry of lights of different colours.—IV. The addition of luminosities of different colour , 1912 .

[42]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Asynchronous processing in vision Color leads motion , 2001, Current Biology.

[43]  R. Andersen,et al.  Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques , 1995, Nature.

[44]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  A paradox of temporal perception revealed by a stimulus oscillating in colour and orientation , 2003, Vision Research.

[45]  N. Mott,et al.  The metal-non-metal transition in nickel sulphide (NiS) , 1971 .

[46]  Michael Harpham December , 1855, The Hospital.

[47]  Colin W G Clifford,et al.  Determinants of asynchronous processing in vision , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Comparison of four methods of heterochromatic photometry. , 1972, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[49]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  A target in real motion appears blurred in the absence of other proximal moving targets , 1995, Vision Research.