Brain circuitries involved in emotional interference task in major depression disorder.

BACKGROUND Emotional and non-emotional Stroop are frequently applied to study major depressive disorder (MDD). The versions of emotional Stroop used in previous studies were not, unlike the ones employed in the present study, based on semantic incongruence, making it difficult to compare the tasks. METHODS We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study the neural and behavioral responses of 18 healthy subjects and 18 subjects with MDD to emotional and non-emotional word-face Stroop tasks based on semantic incompatibility between targets and distractors. RESULTS In both groups, the distractors triggered significant amounts of interference conflict. A between-groups comparison revealed hypoactivation in MDD during emotional task in areas supporting conflict resolution (lateral prefrontal cortex, parietal and extrastriate cortices) paralleled by increased response in the right amygdala. Response in the amygdala, however, did not vary between conflicting and non-conflicting trials. While in the emotional (compared to non-emotional) task healthy controls showed considerably stronger involvement of networks related to conflict resolution, in patients, the processing differences between the two conflict types were negligible. LIMITATIONS The patients group was inhomogeneous in terms of medication and clinical characteristics. The number of female participants was higher, due to which gender effects could not be studied or excluded. CONCLUSION Whilst healthy controls seemed able to adjust the involvement of the network supporting conflict resolution based on conflict demand, patients appeared to lack this capability. The reduced cortical involvement coupled with increased response of limbic structures might underlie the maladjustment vis-à-vis new demands in depressed mood.

[1]  L. Pessoa To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. , 2005, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[3]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control , 2005, NeuroImage.

[4]  Hans-Joachim Mentzel,et al.  Cortical Inefficiency in Patients with Unipolar Depression: An Event-Related fMRI Study with the Stroop Task , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  M. Thase,et al.  Increased Amygdala and Decreased Dorsolateral Prefrontal BOLD Responses in Unipolar Depression: Related and Independent Features , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  A. Anderson,et al.  An fMRI study of stroop word-color interference: evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multiple distributed attentional systems , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  J. Price,et al.  Neurocircuitry of Mood Disorders , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[8]  F. McKenna,et al.  Effects of unattended emotional stimuli on color-naming performance , 1986 .

[9]  H. Wittchen,et al.  SKID I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen. Interviewheft und Beurteilungsheft. Eine deutschsprachige, erweiterte Bearb. d. amerikanischen Originalversion des SKID I , 1997 .

[10]  J. Price,et al.  Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Ute Habel,et al.  Brain Circuitries Involved in Semantic Interference by Demands of Emotional and Non-Emotional Distractors , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  V. Bowden,et al.  The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 1992 .

[13]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[14]  Xiang Wang,et al.  Amygdala hyperactivation and prefrontal hypoactivation in subjects with cognitive vulnerability to depression , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[15]  A. Etkin,et al.  Common abnormalities and disorder-specific compensation during implicit regulation of emotional processing in generalized anxiety and major depressive disorders. , 2011, The American journal of psychiatry.

[16]  George Bush,et al.  The emotional counting stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[17]  Wolfstein,et al.  JOURNAL OF MENTAL SCIENCE , 1905 .

[18]  H. Veiel,et al.  A preliminary profile of neuropsychological deficits associated with major depression. , 1997, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[19]  Gregory A. Miller,et al.  Paying attention to emotion: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[21]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  M. Hamilton,et al.  Factors related to the outcome of depression treated with E.C.T. , 1960, The Journal of mental science.

[23]  T A Ketter,et al.  Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop). , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[25]  R. Bajcsy,et al.  A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies , 2002, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[26]  M E Phelps,et al.  Regional brain metabolic changes in patients with major depression treated with either paroxetine or interpersonal therapy: preliminary findings. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  M. Furey,et al.  Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression , 2008, Brain Structure and Function.

[28]  Nicholas D. Walsh,et al.  Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression , 2007, Psychological Medicine.

[29]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[30]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[31]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  H. Wittchen,et al.  SKID II. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen. Interviewheft. Eine deutschspeachige, erw. Bearb. d. amerikanischen Originalversion d. SKID-II von: M.B. First, R.L. Spitzer, M. Gibbon, J.B.W. Williams, L. Benjamin, (Version 3/96). , 1997 .

[33]  H. Wittchen,et al.  Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview (und Fragebogen) für DSM-IV (SKID-II), Achse II (Persönlichkeitsstörungen). Eine deutschsprachige, erweiterte Bearbeitung der amerikanischen Originalversion des SCID-II von: M.B. First, R.L. Spitzer, M. Gibbon, J.B.W. Williams, L. Benjamin (Version 3/96) , 1996 .

[34]  Jayne-Louise Taylor,et al.  Attentional and Memory Bias in Persecutory Delusions and Depression , 2004, Psychopathology.

[35]  John E. McGeary,et al.  Therapygenetics: moving towards personalized psychotherapy treatment , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Keith M. Shafritz,et al.  The interaction of emotional and cognitive neural systems in emotionally guided response inhibition , 2006, NeuroImage.