The Congruency Sequence Effect 3.0: A Critical Test of Conflict Adaptation

Over the last two decades, the congruency sequence effect (CSE) –the finding of a reduced congruency effect following incongruent trials in conflict tasks– has played a central role in advancing research on cognitive control. According to the influential conflict-monitoring account, the CSE reflects adjustments in selective attention that enhance task focus when needed, often termed conflict adaptation. However, this dominant interpretation of the CSE has been called into question by several alternative accounts that stress the role of episodic memory processes: feature binding and (stimulus-response) contingency learning. To evaluate the notion of conflict adaptation in accounting for the CSE, we construed versions of three widely used experimental paradigms (the colour-word Stroop, picture-word Stroop and flanker task) that effectively control for feature binding and contingency learning. Results revealed that a CSE can emerge in all three tasks. This strongly suggests a contribution of attentional control to the CSE and highlights the potential of these unprecedentedly clean paradigms for further examining cognitive control.

[1]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  B. Hommel Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .

[3]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[4]  E. Soetens,et al.  Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  S. Baron-Cohen A mature view of autism Autism: Explaining the Enigma by Uta Frith, Blackwell Publishing, 2nd Edn, 2003. £15.99 (pbk) (249 pages) ISBN 0 631 22901 9 , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Uwe Mattler,et al.  Delayed flanker effects on lateralized readiness potentials , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  B. Hommel,et al.  A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.

[9]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[10]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[11]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[12]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Peter Wühr,et al.  Evidence for gating of direct response activation in the Simon task. , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Stimulus conflict predicts conflict adaptation in a numerical flanker task , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Dorret I. Boomsma,et al.  Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: Conflict adaptation or associative priming? , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[17]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  The Stroop effect: why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Cognitive control acts locally , 2008, Cognition.

[21]  R. Hartsuiker,et al.  Improving accuracy in detecting acoustic onsets. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. , 2008, Psychological review.

[24]  Avram J. Holmes,et al.  Response conflict and frontocingulate dysfunction in unmedicated participants with major depression , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Sufen Chen,et al.  Sequential effects in the Simon task: Conflict adaptation or feature integration? , 2009, Brain Research.

[26]  E. Awh,et al.  The elusive link between conflict and conflict adaptation , 2009, Psychological research.

[27]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Now you see it, now you don't: controlling for contingencies and stimulus repetitions eliminates the Gratton effect. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[29]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Post-conflict slowing: cognitive adaptation after conflict processing , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Domain-specific conflict adaptation without feature repetitions , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  James R. Schmidt,et al.  Does Temporal Contiguity Moderate Contingency Learning in a Speeded Performance Task? , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  P. Martini,et al.  Context, not conflict, drives cognitive control. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  Conflict adaptation: It is not what you expect , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  J. T. Mordkoff Observation: Three reasons to avoid having half of the trials be congruent in a four-alternative forced-choice experiment on sequential modulation , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[35]  James R. Schmidt Questioning conflict adaptation: proportion congruent and Gratton effects reconsidered , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[36]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Dopaminergic medication counteracts conflict adaptation in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Neuropsychology.

[37]  Wim Notebaert,et al.  The hot-hand fallacy in cognitive control: Repetition expectancy modulates the congruency sequence effect , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  J. Bugg,et al.  Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.