Brain Structures Involved in Visual Search in the Presence and Absence of Color Singletons

It is still debated to what degree top–down and bottom–up driven attentional control processes are subserved by shared or by separate mechanisms. Interactions between these attentional control forms were investigated using a rapid event-related fMRI design, using an attentional search task. Following a prestimulus mask, target stimuli (consisting of a letter C or a mirror image of the C, enclosed in a diamond outline) were presented either at one unique location among three nontarget items (consisting of a random letter, enclosed in a circle outline; 50% probability), or at all four possible target locations (also 50% probability). On half the trials, irrelevant color singletons were presented, consisting of a color change of one of the four prestimulus masks, just prior to target appearance. Participants were required to search for a target letter inside the diamond and report its orientation. Results indicate that, in addition to a common network of parietal areas, medial frontal cortex is uniquely involved in top–down orienting, whereas bottom–up control is mainly subserved by a network of occipital and parietal areas. Additionally, we found that participants who were better able to suppress orienting to the color singleton showed middle frontal gyrus activation, and that the degree of top–down control correlated with insular activity. We conclude that, in addition to a common set of parietal areas, separate brain areas are involved in top–down and bottom–up driven attentional control, and that frontal areas play a role in the suppression of attentional capture by an irrelevant color singleton.

[1]  C. Brunia Neural aspects of anticipatory behavior. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[2]  E. G. Jones Cerebral Cortex , 1987, Cerebral Cortex.

[3]  G. Mangun,et al.  Dissociating top-down attentional control from selective perception and action , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[6]  A. Zeman Attentional Processing. The Brain's Art of Mindfulness , 1996 .

[7]  Jens Schwarzbach,et al.  Control of object-based attention in human cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional Parcellation of Attentional Control Regions of the Brain , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  D. O'Boyle,et al.  The effect of age on inhibition of return is independent of non-ocular response inhibition , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  William R. Harcombe,et al.  From Parasite to Mutualist: Rapid Evolution of Wolbachia in Natural Populations of Drosophila , 2007, PLoS biology.

[11]  K. Hugdahl,et al.  Processing of conflicting cues in an attention-shift paradigm studied with fMRI , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Neural Correlates of Attentional Capture in Visual Search , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional capture and inhibition (of return): The effect on perceptual sensitivity , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  Marty G Woldorff,et al.  Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention , 2007, PLoS biology.

[16]  Lihua Mao,et al.  Neural correlates of covert orienting of visual spatial attention along vertical and horizontal dimensions , 2007, Brain Research.

[17]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  A M Dale,et al.  Randomized event‐related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[19]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations: commentary on Pratt and Abrams (1995). , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Thomas J. Ross,et al.  Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom–up and top–down processes of visuospatial selective attention , 2006, NeuroImage.

[22]  L. M. Warner,et al.  The Neural Mechanisms for Minimizing Cross-Modal Distraction , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Woldorff,et al.  Distortion of ERP averages due to overlap from temporally adjacent ERPs: analysis and correction. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[24]  David LaBerge,et al.  Attentional Processing: The Brain's Art of Mindfulness , 1995 .

[25]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Capturing attention , 1981, Cognition.

[26]  G. Mangun,et al.  Author ' s personal copy Research Report fMRI evidence for both generalized and specialized components of attentional control , 2007 .

[27]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Why do visual offsets reduce saccadic latencies? , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[29]  S. Yantis,et al.  Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Neural Basis of Endogenous and Exogenous Spatial Orienting: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Douglas P. Munoz,et al.  Neurophysiological Correlates of the Reflexive Orienting of Spatial Attention , 2005 .

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  S. Yantis,et al.  Spatially selective representations of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional priority in human occipital, parietal, and frontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  fMRI measurements of color in macaque and human. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[35]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Faster, more intense! The relation between electrophysiological reflections of attentional orienting, sensory gain control, and speed of responding , 2007, Brain Research.

[36]  G. Mangun,et al.  ERP and fMRI measures of visual spatial selective attention , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[37]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Are eyes special? It depends on how you look at it , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  Joseph B. Hopfinger,et al.  Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[39]  R. Klein,et al.  Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Fang Fang,et al.  Perceptual grouping and inverse fMRI activity patterns in human visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[41]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Neurobiology of Attention , 2005 .

[42]  D. LaBerge,et al.  Attention, consciousness, and electrical wave activity within the cortical column. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[43]  H. Garavan,et al.  Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[44]  S. Yantis,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  T. Paus,et al.  Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[48]  N. Lavie,et al.  Frontal control of attentional capture in visual search , 2006 .

[49]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.

[50]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[51]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[52]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the same large-scale neural network , 2004, NeuroImage.

[53]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[54]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[55]  Teemu Rinne,et al.  Distributed cortical networks for focused auditory attention and distraction , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[56]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  G Mulder,et al.  Working memory processes show different degrees of lateralization: evidence from event-related potentials. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[58]  J. Pratt,et al.  Inhibition of return to successively cued spatial locations. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[59]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.