Self and team prioritisation effects in perceptual matching: Evidence for a shared representation.

Previous research has demonstrated that in-group favouritism occurs not only in higher-level judgments such as reward allocation, but also in low-level perceptual and attentional tasks. Recently, Moradi, Sui, Hewstone, and Humphreys (2015) found a novel effect of in-group bias on a simple perceptual matching task in which football fans responded more efficiently to stimuli newly associated with their own football team than stimuli associated with rival or neutral teams. This result is consistent with a robust self-bias effect in which individuals show a large performance advantage in responding to stimuli associated with the self over stimuli associated with a close friend or a stranger (Sui, He, & Humphreys, 2012). The present research utilised a perceptual matching paradigm to investigate the relations between self and in-group prioritisation amongst a sample of college rowers. Across two experiments, we demonstrated a reliable performance advantage for self and team stimuli. We also found a relationship between the self and team advantage in RT, and demonstrated an overlap in the perception of self- and team-associated shapes that was stronger in participants who reported a greater sense of group identity with their team. Further, we found no relation between the team bias and positive valence implicitly associated with the team, showing that the team bias effects are unlikely to be driven by emotional significance. The results are consistent with an overlap between self and in-group representation, which may provide evidence for a common process driving both self and in-group perceptual advantage effects.

[1]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Race Categorization Modulates Holistic Face Encoding , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  Brittany S. Cassidy,et al.  Social relevance enhances memory for impressions in older adults , 2012, Memory.

[3]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  M. Hogg,et al.  Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. , 1989 .

[5]  J. Brigham,et al.  The Influence of Race on Eyewitness Memory , 2007 .

[6]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Interaction between Self-Bias and Reward: Evidence for Common and Distinct Processes , 2015, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[8]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  An In-Group Becomes Part of the Self: Response Time Evidence , 1996 .

[9]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Overlapping Mental Representations of Self and In-Group: Reaction Time Evidence and Its Relationship with Explicit Measures of Group Identification☆ , 2000 .

[10]  M. Conway,et al.  The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. , 2000, Psychological review.

[11]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Perceptual effects of social salience: evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Shihui Han,et al.  Cultural difference in neural mechanisms of self-recognition , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[13]  A. Vázquez,et al.  The power of ‘feeling one’ with a group: identity fusion and extreme pro-group behaviours / El poder de ‘sentirse uno’ con un grupo: fusión de la identidad y conductas progrupales extremas , 2015 .

[14]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Experiments in intergroup discrimination. , 1970, Scientific American.

[15]  Jolanda Jetten,et al.  On the nature of identity fusion: insights into the construct and a new measure. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  H. Tajfel Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations , 1982 .

[17]  G. Humphreys,et al.  In-group modulation of perceptual matching , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Signal detection theory as data analysis method and psychological decision model , 1993 .

[19]  Xiaoying Wang,et al.  Do You Feel My Pain? Racial Group Membership Modulates Empathic Neural Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[21]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The automatic and the expected self: separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Jeroen Vaes,et al.  The Emotional Side of Prejudice: The Attribution of Secondary Emotions to Ingroups and Outgroups , 2000 .

[23]  Bertjan Doosje,et al.  Group-level self-definition and self-investment: a hierarchical (multicomponent) model of in-group identification. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  A. Aron,et al.  Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness , 1992 .

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[26]  James D. Johnson,et al.  Rodney King and O. J. Revisited: The Impact of Race and Defendant Empathy Induction on Judicial Decisions , 2002 .

[27]  Harald T. Schupp,et al.  Emotional Facilitation of Sensory Processing in the Visual Cortex , 2003, Psychological science.

[28]  N. Cowan,et al.  The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: how frequent are attention shifts to one's name in an irrelevant auditory channel? , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Overlapping Mental Representations of Self, In-Group, and Partner: Further Response Time Evidence and a Connectionist Model , 1999 .

[30]  Jolanda Jetten,et al.  When group membership gets personal: a theory of identity fusion. , 2012, Psychological review.

[31]  M. Brewer The Social Self: On Being the Same and Different at the Same Time , 1991 .

[32]  S. Yantis,et al.  Persistence of value-driven attentional capture. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  G. Northoff,et al.  Is Our Self Nothing but Reward? , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[34]  N. Brady,et al.  Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, more accurate performance which persists when faces are inverted , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  Ying Zhu,et al.  Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[36]  David J. Turk,et al.  Yours or mine? Ownership and memory , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[37]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Dissociating Biases towards the Self and Positive Emotion , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  G Wolford,et al.  Processing of unattended visual information , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[40]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Social categorization and intergroup behaviour , 1971 .

[41]  S. Bishop,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Selective Attention to Threat , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[42]  David J. Turk,et al.  Self-memory biases in explicit and incidental encoding of trait adjectives , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.