Comparing P300 modulations: target-to-target interval versus infrequent nontarget-to-nontarget interval in a three-stimulus task.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Michelle L Brennan | Robert J Barry | C. Gonsalvez | R. Barry | G. Steiner | Genevieve Z Steiner | Craig J Gonsalvez
[1] J. Polich,et al. P300 from auditory and somatosensory stimuli: probability and inter-stimulus interval. , 1991, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[2] I. Jentzsch,et al. Sequence-sensitive subcomponents of P300: topographical analyses and dipole source localization. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[3] N. Squires,et al. The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.
[4] C. Gonsalvez,et al. Target-to-target interval versus probability effects on P300 in one- and two-tone tasks. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[5] Andrew Heathcote,et al. Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[6] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[7] J. Cohen,et al. P300, stimulus intensity, modality, and probability. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[8] J. Polich. Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[9] R. Verleger. Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] E Gordon,et al. Is the target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[11] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[12] E. Donchin,et al. Probability effects on stimulus evaluation and response processes. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] J. Polich,et al. P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[14] E. Donchin,et al. On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[15] Vince D Calhoun,et al. fMRI in an oddball task: effects of target-to-target interval. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[16] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[17] Ray Johnson. For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .
[18] M K Kaiser,et al. MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: an extensive primer. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[19] J. Polich,et al. P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. , 1990, Psychophysiology.
[20] T W Picton,et al. Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.
[21] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[22] E. Donchin,et al. On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.
[23] E Donchin,et al. P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[24] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[25] R. Verleger. On the utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. , 1997, Psychophysiology.
[26] Evian Gordon,et al. Numbers of preceding nontargets differentially affect responses to targets in normal volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: A study of event-related potentials , 1995, Psychiatry Research.
[27] A. Engel,et al. What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[28] C. Gonsalvez,et al. Target-to-target interval, intensity, and P300 from an auditory single-stimulus task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[29] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[30] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[31] R. Johnson. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[32] E. Donchin,et al. The influence of stimulus deviance and novelty on the P300 and novelty P3. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[33] W. Pritchard. Psychophysiology of P300. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.
[34] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The effects of stimulus sequence on event related potentials: A comparison of visual and auditory sequences , 1977 .
[35] T W Picton,et al. The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[36] Emanuel Donchin,et al. The P300 component of the event-related brain potential as an index of information processing , 1982, Biological Psychology.
[37] H. Jasper,et al. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.