Causal explanation as constraint satisfaction : A critique and a feedforward connectionist alternative

S. J. Read and A. Marcus-Newhall (1993) proposed that constraint satisfaction connectionist models can account for four principles of coherence that underlie social explanation. However, it is argued that the specific implementation they used (ECHO; P. Thagard, 1992) has several important shortcomings. ECHO fails to be sensitive to covariation that is crucial to causality, it does not learn from experience, and it cannot deal efficiently with conjunctions of explanations. To remedy these shortcomings, the author presents an alternative feedforward connectionist model that prior research has shown to be susceptible to the classical attribution principles of covariation. discounting, and augmentation. By running novel simulations of Read and Marcus-Newhall's data, the author demonstrated that this feedforward model is also capable of simulating the four principles of coherence.

[1]  T. Shultz,et al.  Cognitive dissonance reduction as constraint satisfaction. , 1996, Psychological review.

[2]  David R. Shanks,et al.  CATEGORIZATION BY A CONNECTIONIST NETWORK , 1991 .

[3]  H. Kelley Attribution in social interaction. , 1987 .

[4]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Assimilating evidence: The key to revision? , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  William K. Estes,et al.  Classification and cognition , 1994 .

[6]  H. Kelley Causal schemata and the attribution process , 1972 .

[7]  S. Read,et al.  Explanatory coherence in social explanations : a parallel distributed processing account , 1993 .

[8]  J. Pearce Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. , 1994, Psychological review.

[9]  F. Vallée-Tourangeau,et al.  Selective associations and causality judgments: Presence of a strong causal factor may reduce judgments of a weaker one. , 1993 .

[10]  S. Read,et al.  Rapist or "Regular Guy": Explanatory Coherence in the Construction of Mental Models of Others , 1993 .

[11]  D. Shanks Forward and Backward Blocking in Human Contingency Judgement , 1985 .

[12]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Dispositional Attributions Require the Joint Application of the Methods of Difference and Agreement , 1997 .

[13]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  A Coherence Model of Cognitive Consistency: Dynamics of Attitude Change During the Persian Gulf War , 1993 .

[14]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[15]  R. D. Hansen,et al.  Discounting and augmenting facilitative and inhibitory forces: The winner takes almost all. , 1985 .

[16]  P. Thagard,et al.  Explanatory coherence , 1993 .

[17]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Explorations in parallel distributed processing: a handbook of models, programs, and exercises , 1988 .

[18]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  The handbook of brain theory and neural networks , 1995, A Bradford book.

[19]  Alan M. Lesgold,et al.  Coherence: Beyond constraint satisfaction , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[21]  Eliot R. Smith What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other , 1996 .

[22]  B. Weiner "Spontaneous" causal thinking. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  W. Estes,et al.  Base-rate effects in category learning: a comparison of parallel network and memory storage-retrieval models. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Cognitive organization of impressions: effects of incongruency in complex representations. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  G. Chapman,et al.  Trial order affects cue interaction in contingency judgment. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  P. Thagard,et al.  FORMING IMPRESSIONS FROM STEREOTYPES, TRAITS, AND BEHAVIORS : A PARALLEL-CONSTRAINT-SATISFACTION THEORY , 1996 .

[27]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition : Psychological and Biological Models , 1986 .

[28]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[29]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[30]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Cue Competition in Causality Judgments: The Role of Nonpresentation of Compound Stimulus Elements , 1994 .

[31]  D. Shanks Is Human Learning Rational? , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[32]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Schemata and Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models , 1986 .

[33]  G. Chapman,et al.  Cue interaction in human contingency judgment , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[34]  G. Bower,et al.  Evaluating an adaptive network model of human learning , 1988 .

[35]  D. Shanks On Similarities between Causal Judgments in Experienced and Described Situations , 1991 .

[36]  D. Shanks,et al.  Human instrumental learning: a critical review of data and theory. , 1993, British journal of psychology.

[37]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Assessment of the Rescorla-Wagner model. , 1995 .

[38]  E. E. Jones Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior , 1987 .

[39]  P. Cheng,et al.  A probabilistic contrast model of causal induction. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Covariation, discounting, and augmentation: Towards a clarification of attributional principles , 1978 .

[41]  G. Bower,et al.  From conditioning to category learning: an adaptive network model. , 1988 .

[42]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.