The role of cortical sensorimotor oscillations in action anticipation

Abstract The human mirror neuron system is believed to play an important role in facilitating the ability of athletes to anticipate the actions of an opponent. This system is often assessed with EEG by measuring event‐related changes in mu (8–13 Hz) sensorimotor oscillations. However, traditional channel‐based analyses of this measure are flawed in that due to volume conduction effects mu and non‐mu alpha activity can become mixed. This flaw means it is unclear the extent to which mu activity indexes the mirror system, as opposed to other processes such as attentional demand. As a solution to this problem, we use independent component analysis to separate out the underlying brain processes during a tennis‐related action observation and anticipation task. We investigated expertise‐related differences in independent component activity. Experienced tennis players (N=18) were significantly more accurate than unexperienced novices (N=21) on the anticipation task. EEG results found significant group differences in both the mu and beta (15–25 Hz) frequency bands in sensorimotor components, with earlier and greater desynchronisation in the experienced tennis players. In particular, only experienced players showed desynchronisation in the high mu (11–13 Hz) band. No group differences were found in posterior alpha components. These results show for the first time that expertise differences during action observation and anticipation are unique to sensorimotor sources, and that no expertise‐related differences exist in attention modulated, posterior alpha sources. As such, this paper provides a much cleaner measure of the human mirror system during action observation, and its modulation by motor expertise, than has been possible in previous work. HighlightsInvestigated the role of the human mirror system in skilled action anticipation.Experienced and unexperienced tennis players performed a tennis anticipation task.Used ICA to separate sensorimotor mu processes from non‐mu alpha activity.Results show group differences were unique to sensorimotor mu activity.Non‐mu alpha activity, that are modulated by attention, did not differ between groups.

[1]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[2]  I. Toni,et al.  Distinct roles for alpha- and beta-band oscillations during mental simulation of goal-directed actions. , 2014, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  C. Buss,et al.  Children's Brain Development Benefits from Longer Gestation , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[6]  R. Miall,et al.  Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. , 2003, Neuroreport.

[7]  H. Fukuyama,et al.  Alpha-band desynchronization in human parietal area during reach planning , 2012, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  David I. Donaldson,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Making the Case for Mobile Cognition: Eeg and Sports Performance , 2022 .

[9]  D. Bishop,et al.  Mu suppression – A good measure of the human mirror neuron system? , 2016, Cortex.

[10]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Human Brain Dynamics Accompanying Use of Egocentric and Allocentric Reference Frames during Navigation , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Williams,et al.  Recognizing and predicting movement effects: identifying critical movement features. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[12]  F. McKenna,et al.  Skilled anticipation in real-world tasks: measurement of attentional demands in the domain of tennis. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[13]  James M Kilner,et al.  What is simulated in the action observation network when we observe actions? , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  K. Ericsson,et al.  Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise: an introduction , 1991 .

[15]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  G. Knoblich,et al.  The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  Rudolf Stark,et al.  Prediction of human actions: Expertise and task‐related effects on neural activation of the action observation network , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[19]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Electroencephalographic Brain Dynamics Following Manually Responded Visual Targets , 2004, PLoS biology.

[21]  P. Rossini,et al.  Alpha, beta and gamma electrocorticographic rhythms in somatosensory, motor, premotor and prefrontal cortical areas differ in movement execution and observation in humans , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  A. Williams,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Sports Sciences Visual Search, Anticipation and Expertise in Soccer Goalkeepers Visual Search, Anticipation and Expertise in Soccer Goalkeepers , 2022 .

[23]  S. Makeig,et al.  Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Giuseppe Pellizzer,et al.  Brain oscillatory activity during motor preparation: effect of directional uncertainty on beta, but not alpha, frequency band , 2015, Front. Neurosci..

[26]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Williams,et al.  Global Information Pickup Underpins Anticipation of Tennis Shot Direction , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  Ross E. Vanderwert,et al.  Action Experience, More than Observation, Influences Mu Rhythm Desynchronization , 2014, PloS one.

[29]  Philipp M. Keune,et al.  Winning the game: brain processes in expert, young elite and amateur table tennis players , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[30]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  Ivo Käthner,et al.  An auditory multiclass brain-computer interface with natural stimuli: Usability evaluation with healthy participants and a motor impaired end user , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Dynamic Brain Sources of Visual Evoked Responses , 2002, Science.

[33]  M. Endsley Situation Models: An Avenue to the Modeling of Mental Models , 2000 .

[34]  K. Davids,et al.  Declarative knowledge in sport: a by-product of experience or a characteristic of expertise? , 1995 .

[35]  Vincent Walsh Is sport the brain’s biggest challenge? , 2014, Current Biology.

[36]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[37]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Brain regions concerned with the identification of deceptive soccer moves by higher-skilled and lower-skilled players , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[38]  Kathryn H. Yoo,et al.  Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta-analysis. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. , 2005, Human movement science.

[40]  René J. Huster,et al.  Methods for Simultaneous EEG-fMRI: An Introductory Review , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Functional MRI reveals expert-novice differences during sport-related anticipation , 2010, Neuroreport.

[42]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  Single-Trial Normalization for Event-Related Spectral Decomposition Reduces Sensitivity to Noisy Trials , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[43]  Jean Decety,et al.  Intention understanding over T: a neuroimaging study on shared representations and tennis return predictions , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[44]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Independent EEG Sources Are Dipolar , 2012, PloS one.

[45]  S. Makris Sport neuroscience revisited (?): a commentary , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[46]  L. Braadbaart,et al.  Do mirror neuron areas mediate mu rhythm suppression during imitation and action observation? , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[47]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[49]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: implications for applied cognitive psychology , 2011 .

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Cortical mechanisms underlying the organization of goal-directed actions and mirror neuron-based action understanding. , 2014, Physiological reviews.

[52]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Cortical fMRI activation to opponents’ body kinematics in sport-related anticipation: Expert-novice differences with normal and point-light video , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[53]  A. Engel,et al.  What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[54]  A. Leuthold,et al.  Beta-Band Activity during Motor Planning Reflects Response Uncertainty , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  E. Olivier,et al.  Pupil size variations correlate with physical effort perception , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[56]  Claire Braboszcz,et al.  Stability of ICA decomposition across within-subject EEG datasets , 2012, 2012 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[57]  James M. Kilner,et al.  A New Framework to Explain Sensorimotor Beta Oscillations , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  M. Heil,et al.  Expertise in dance modulates alpha/beta event‐related desynchronization during action observation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[59]  W. Klimesch,et al.  What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[60]  N. Troje,et al.  Exploring motor system contributions to the perception of social information: Evidence from EEG activity in the mu/alpha frequency range , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[61]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[62]  F. McKenna,et al.  The Effect of Disguise on Novice and Expert Tennis Players’ Anticipation Ability , 2009 .

[63]  N. Hagemann,et al.  The dynamical information underpinning anticipation skill. , 2009, Human movement science.

[64]  Rudolf Stark,et al.  The influence of expertise on brain activation of the action observation network during anticipation of tennis and volleyball serves , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[65]  Kelvin S. Oie,et al.  Cognition in action: imaging brain/body dynamics in mobile humans , 2011, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[66]  Claudio Del Percio,et al.  “Neural efficiency” of athletes’ brain for upright standing: A high-resolution EEG study , 2009, Brain Research Bulletin.

[67]  U. Castiello,et al.  Motor resonance in left- and right-handers: evidence for effector-independent motor representations , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[68]  Martin J. McKeown,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts: comparison between ICA and PCA , 1998, Neural Networks for Signal Processing VIII. Proceedings of the 1998 IEEE Signal Processing Society Workshop (Cat. No.98TH8378).

[69]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[70]  S. Bentin,et al.  Mirror activity in the human brain while observing hand movements: A comparison between EEG desynchronization in the μ-range and previous fMRI results , 2009, Brain Research.

[71]  Eran Dayan,et al.  Alpha and Beta Band Event-Related Desynchronization Reflects Kinematic Regularities , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[73]  J. Pineda The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[74]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Functional dissociation of lower and upper frequency mu rhythms in relation to voluntary limb movement , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[75]  J. Faubert,et al.  Soccer athletes are superior to non-athletes at perceiving soccer-specific and non-sport specific human biological motion , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[76]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Expertise and Attunement to Kinematic Constraints , 2008, Perception.

[77]  Mica R. Endsley,et al.  Toward a Theory of Situation Awareness in Dynamic Systems , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[78]  S. Makeig Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.