Procedural motor learning in Parkinson's disease

Abstract. We have been investigating motor control and learning in parkinsonian subjects. In the current study, we sought to explore the existence of deficits in procedural motor learning, which is a form of implicit motor learning where skill improves over repetitive blocks of trials. We sought to determine, in particular, whether any such deficit is accentuated during specific types or phases of learning. We would expect that those specific learning tasks would require the greatest participation of the basal ganglia. Numerous studies have found that Parkinson's disease (PD) patients may show deficits in learning. Combined with information about basal ganglia neuronal connections and activity, this led some investigators to suggest that one of the key functions of the basal ganglia is to facilitate learning. To investigate these learning deficits, we used a robotic device to generate conservative force fields that disturbed the subjects' arm movements, thereby generating a "virtual mechanical environment" that subjects learned to manipulate. Movements were successively grouped into blocks comprising five different conditions: motor performance, early learning, late learning, negative transfer, and aftereffect motor performance. Our results with eight right-handed PD subjects and nine age-matched controls showed a relative decrease in the rate of learning for the PD patients in all blocks, but greater differences emerged between groups during novelty phases of learning. In particular, the difference in performance during the negative transfer condition reached statistical significance, suggesting that the basal ganglia might be a key center for "switching" motor patterns. Our results support the hypothesis that deficiencies in procedural motor learning are characteristic of PD. They add to existing evidence which has suggested a key role for the basal ganglia when new sensorimotor mappings are required by novel task environments. Better understanding of these deficits should facilitate the rehabilitation of PD patients.

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  M. Hallett,et al.  Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.

[3]  Fel'dman Ag,et al.  A model of central regulation of movement parameters , 1984 .

[4]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  C. Frith,et al.  Impairments in the learning and performance of a new manual skill in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1986, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[6]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[7]  J. Schneider,et al.  Basal ganglia and behavior : sensory aspects of motor functioning , 1987 .

[8]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  C. Marsden,et al.  Prism adaptation and other tasks involving spatial abilities in patients with Parkinson's disease, patients with frontal lobe lesions and patients with unilateral temporal lobectomies , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Practice effects on the preprogramming of discrete movements in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  E. Marder,et al.  Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[12]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  C. Marsden,et al.  Cognitive function in Parkinson's disease: From description to theory , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, "prefrontal" and "limbic" functions. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[15]  M. Hallett,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[16]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of procedural motor learning: Relating cerebral blood flow with individual subject performance , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[18]  M. Berkinblit,et al.  Influence of movement speed on accuracy of pointing to memorized targets in 3D space , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  G. E. Alexander Basal Ganglia‐Thalamocortical Circuits: Their Role in Control of Movements , 1994, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[20]  A M Graybiel,et al.  The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. , 1994, Science.

[21]  E. Tulving,et al.  Memory Systems 1994 , 1994 .

[22]  A. Graybiel Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  K. V. Baev,et al.  Disturbances of learning processes in the basal ganglia in the pathogenesis of Parkinson's disease: a novel theory. , 1995, Neurological research.

[24]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[26]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  C Caltagirone,et al.  Implicit memory in parkinsonian patients: evidence for deficient skill learning. , 1996, European neurology.

[28]  Y. Agid,et al.  Memory for spatial location is affected in Parkinson's disease , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  H. Allain,et al.  Assessment of procedural memory in parkinson's disease , 1996, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[30]  Integration of robotic technology with functional imaging , 1996, NeuroImage.

[31]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Motor skill learning in Parkinson's disease , 1996, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[32]  J. Houk On the role of the cerebellum and basal ganglia in cognitive signal processing. , 1997, Progress in brain research.

[33]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[34]  N. Alpert,et al.  Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning. , 1997, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[35]  Richard lvry,et al.  Cerebellar timing systems. , 1997 .

[36]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[37]  C. Marsden,et al.  Learning manual pursuit tracking skills in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  R. Ivry Cerebellar timing systems. , 1997, International review of neurobiology.

[39]  J. Houk,et al.  Network models of the basal ganglia , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Robot-aided neuro-rehabilitation and functional imaging , 1997 .

[41]  N. Hogan,et al.  The effect of robot-assisted therapy and rehabilitative training on motor recovery following stroke. , 1997, Archives of neurology.

[42]  G. Berns,et al.  Brain regions responsive to novelty in the absence of awareness. , 1997, Science.

[43]  M. Smith,et al.  Distorted visual feedback effects on drawing in Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[44]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination in patients with Parkinson’s disease: motor learning deficits and the importance of augmented information feedback , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  R. Siegert,et al.  Implicit learning in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a verbal version of the serial reaction time task. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[46]  A. Graybiel The Basal Ganglia and Chunking of Action Repertoires , 1998, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[47]  E. Vakil,et al.  Declarative and Procedural Learning in Parkinson's Disease Patients Having Tremor or Bradykinesia as the Predominant Symptom , 1998, Cortex.

[48]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  N. Hogan,et al.  Robot‐aided functional imaging: Application to a motor learning study , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[50]  N. Hogan,et al.  Robot-aided neurorehabilitation. , 1998, IEEE transactions on rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[51]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A Computational Model of How the Basal Ganglia Produce Sequences , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  M. Jüptner,et al.  A review of differences between basal ganglia and cerebellar control of movements as revealed by functional imaging studies. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  T. Ebner A role for the cerebellum in the control of limb movement velocity , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[54]  J. Houk,et al.  Model of cortical-basal ganglionic processing: encoding the serial order of sensory events. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Berkinblit,et al.  Pointing to remembered targets in 3-D space in Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Motor control.

[56]  J. Kropotov,et al.  Selection of actions in the basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: review and model. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[57]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Learning in Parkinson’s disease: eyeblink conditioning, declarative learning, and procedural learning , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[58]  N. Hogan,et al.  Quantization of continuous arm movements in humans with brain injury. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  Hermano Igo Krebs,et al.  Arm Movement Control is both Continuous and Discrete , 1999 .

[60]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[61]  N. Hogan,et al.  Robot training enhanced motor outcome in patients with stroke maintained over 3 years , 1999, Neurology.

[62]  N. Hogan,et al.  A novel approach to stroke rehabilitation , 2000, Neurology.

[63]  E. Vakil,et al.  Motor and non-motor sequence learning in patients with basal ganglia lesions: the case of serial reaction time (SRT) , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  What and When: Parallel and Convergent Processing in Motor Control , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  M. Mauk,et al.  Cerebellar function: Coordination, learning or timing? , 2000, Current Biology.

[66]  J L van Hemmen,et al.  Time window control: a model for cerebellar function based on synchronization, reverberation, and time slicing. , 2000, Progress in brain research.

[67]  D. Brooks Imaging basal ganglia function , 2000, Journal of anatomy.

[68]  J. Sage,et al.  The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets in Parkinson’s disease , 2001, Neuroscience.