The social and cultural context of stone knapping skill acquisition

in which humans live shape our opportunities for action and learning at a fundamental level, providing a cumulative ‘ratchet effect’ that may underlie many of the distinctive cognitive skills of modern humans (Tomasello 1999). Understanding the origins and history of this uniquely human mode of cultural existence is a central question for human-evolutionary studies. One potential source of insight into this question lies in the physical traces of proto-human behaviour, and particularly in the durable stone artefacts that dominate the archaeological record. In order to be useful, however, these artefacts must be carefully interpreted using actualistic models based on observable phenomena in the modern world. Artefact replication and other experimental approaches have provided valuable insights into knapping techniques, tool functions and site formation processes (review in Toth 1991) and are even beginning to elucidate the neural foundations of knapping skill (Stout et al. 2000; Stout this volume). However, experimental approaches are ill suited to exploring the cultural dimensions of stone knapping. For this, an ethnographic perspective is needed. Dietrich Stout

[1]  C. Karlin,et al.  The ancient mind: Prehistoric technology: a cognitive science? , 1994 .

[2]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  Willard Van Orman Quine,et al.  Word and Object , 1960 .

[4]  Alan Fogel,et al.  Information, creativity, and culture. , 1997 .

[5]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1997, Journal of comparative psychology.

[6]  Julie C. Stout,et al.  Stone Tool-Making and Brain Activation: Position Emission Tomography (PET) Studies , 2000 .

[7]  G. Dietrich,et al.  Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone‐bead knapping in Khambhat, India , 1995 .

[8]  Pierre Pétrequin,et al.  From polished stone tool to sacred axe: the axes of the Danis of Irian Jaya, Indonesia , 1993 .

[9]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[10]  K. Brown An Alternative Approach to Cognition in the Lower Palaeolithic: The Modular View , 1993, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[11]  A. Collins,et al.  Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning , 1989 .

[12]  R. Pea Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education , 1993 .

[13]  John C. Whittaker,et al.  Flintknapping: Making and Understanding Stone Tools , 1994 .

[14]  J. D. Clark,et al.  A Study of the Work of a Modern Tanner in Ethiopia and Its Relevance for Archaeological Interpretation , 1981 .

[15]  A. Jonker Origins of the modern mind. Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition , 1998 .

[16]  Anne E. Russon Between Culture and Biology: Comparative developmental perspectives on culture: the great apes , 2002 .

[17]  R. Holloway, Culture: A Human Domain , 1969, Current Anthropology.

[18]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Great Ape Societies: Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use , 1996 .

[19]  D. Perkins,et al.  Partners in Cognition: Extending Human Intelligence with Intelligent Technologies , 1991 .

[20]  C. Boesch Teaching among wild chimpanzees , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  T. Wynn Archaeology and cognitive evolution. , 2002, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[22]  Andrew Strathern,et al.  The Rope of Moka: Big-men and Ceremonial Exchange in Mount Hagen New Guinea , 1971 .

[23]  K. Weedman An ethnoarchaeological study of stone scrapers among the Gamo people of southern Ethiopia , 2000 .

[24]  D. Stout Skill and Cognition in Stone Tool Production , 2002, Current Anthropology.

[25]  O. W. Hampton Culture of Stone: Sacred and Profane Uses of Stone among the Dani , 1999 .

[26]  Anne-Nelly Perret-Clermont,et al.  The social construction of meaning and cognitive activity in elementary school children , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[27]  C. Geertz The interpretation of cultures / Clifford Geertz , 1973 .

[28]  B. Rogoff Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context , 1990 .

[29]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[30]  Elena L. Grigorenko,et al.  Practical Intelligence in Everyday Life , 2000 .

[31]  T. Wynn The Evolution of Spatial Competence , 1989 .

[32]  J. Bruner,et al.  The role of tutoring in problem solving. , 1976, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[33]  Etienne Wenger,et al.  Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation , 1991 .

[34]  D. K. Feil,et al.  The evolution of highland Papua New Guinea societies , 1989 .

[35]  Deborah Olausson Different Strokes for Different Folks. Possible Reasons for Variation in Quality of Knapping , 1998 .

[36]  Barry S. Hewlett,et al.  Cultural Transmission Among Aka Pygmies , 1986 .

[37]  Kim Hill,et al.  A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity , 2000 .

[38]  L. Vygotsky Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press , 1978 .

[39]  Beryl Lieff Benderly,et al.  The growth of the mind : and the endangered origins of intelligence , 1997 .

[40]  R. Martin,et al.  The Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution , 1994 .

[41]  Robert J. Crutcher,et al.  The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. , 1993 .

[42]  Michelle Y. Merrill,et al.  The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. , 1999, Journal of human evolution.

[43]  L. S. Vygotksy Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[44]  Nicholas Toth,et al.  THE LAST STONE AX MAKERS , 1992 .