Neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic and lacteal fluids: a test of perinatal chemosensory continuity.

The head-orientation response of 2- and 4-day-old breast-feeding neonates was studied in paired-choice odor tests. Three tests were conducted on day 2 (amniotic fluid [AF] versus Colostrum; AF versus Control; Colostrum versus Control) and on day 4 (AF versus Milk; AF versus Control; Milk versus Control). At 2 days, both AF and Colostrum elicited positive orientation when presented simultaneously with the control stimulus, indicating that both odors were detectable to the infants. However, no differential responses were noted when AF and colostrum were presented concurrently, suggesting that both of these substrates were treated as similar sensorily and/or hedonically. On day 4, the odors of AF and transitional milk elicited attraction responses when presented in competition with a control stimulus. When the odor of milk was presented simultaneously with the odor of AF, the former elicited longer head orientation. Thus, within the first 4 days of life olfactory selectivity changes from a null preference between cues carried in AF and in colostrum to a positive preference for cues carried in postamniotic odors, that is, breast milk. An additional experiment indicated that 3-day-old neonates orient longer toward the odor of their own AF than toward the odor of alien AF, showing that prenatal odors elicit selective responding for some time after birth. Altogether these results were interpreted as supporting the hypotheses that prenatal experience might influence the earliest odor preferences in the breast-feeding human neonate and that these preferences rapidly evolve according to postnatal experience.

[1]  Morten Meilgaard,et al.  Sensory Evaluation Techniques , 2020 .

[2]  A Macfarlane,et al.  Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. , 2008, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[3]  P. Orgeur,et al.  Olfaction in Utero: Can the Rodent Model be Generalized? , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  M. Horning,et al.  Profiles of volatile metabolites in body fluids , 1976 .

[5]  I. G. Macy Composition of human colostrum and milk. , 1949, American journal of diseases of children.

[6]  Lucas P. J. J. Noldus,et al.  The Observer: A software system for collection and analysis of observational data , 1991 .

[7]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Garlic ingestion by pregnant women alters the odor of amniotic fluid. , 1995, Chemical senses.

[8]  M. Russell,et al.  Human olfactory communication , 1976, Nature.

[9]  R. Porter,et al.  Breast-fed infants respond to olfactory cues from their own mother and unfamiliar lactating females☆ , 1992 .

[10]  G. Turkewitz THE DEVELOPMENT OF LATERAL DIFFERENTIATION IN THE HUMAN INFANT , 1977, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  J. Baum Parent--offspring relations in man. , 1981, Journal of reproduction and fertility.

[12]  P. Hartmann,et al.  Changes in human milk composition during the initiation of lactation. , 1981, The Australian journal of experimental biology and medical science.

[13]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  The human infants' response to vanilla flavors in mother's milk and formula , 1996 .

[14]  R. Porter,et al.  Recognition of maternal axillary odors by infants. , 1985, Child development.

[15]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Evidence suggesting that the odortypes of pregnant women are a compound of maternal and fetal odortypes. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  A. Gilbert,et al.  Sex, handedness and side of nose modulate human odor perception , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  E. Blass,et al.  Prenatal and postnatal determinants of the 1st suckling episode in albino rats. , 1982, Developmental psychobiology.

[18]  R. Hudson,et al.  Transmission of food preference in the rabbit: The means of information transfer , 1994, Physiology & Behavior.

[19]  C. Arnould,et al.  Olfactory preferences in newborn lambs: possible influence of prenatal experience. , 1995 .

[20]  R. Porter,et al.  Olfactory preferences resulting from mere exposure in human neonates , 1986 .

[21]  P. Hepper The amniotic fluid: an important priming role in kin recognition , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  M. Kinsbourne,et al.  The mechanism of neonatal rightward turning bias: A sensory or motor asymmetry? , 1980 .

[23]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds , 1986 .

[24]  M. Schleidt,et al.  The significance of mother's perfume for infants in the first weeks of their life , 1990 .

[25]  Gerd Kobal,et al.  Differences in human chemosensory evoked potentials to olfactory and somatosensory chemical stimuli presented to left and right nostrils , 1992 .

[26]  H. Montagner,et al.  Les stimulations olfactives dans les relations entre l'enfant et la mère , 1980 .

[27]  A Yonas,et al.  Radial localization of odors by human newborns. , 1976, Child development.

[28]  R. Porter,et al.  Attractiveness of lactating females' breast odors to neonates. , 1989, Child development.

[29]  R. Soussignan,et al.  Bottle-fed neonates prefer an odor experienced in utero to an odor experienced postnatally in the feeding context. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[30]  D. Coppola,et al.  Olfaction in utero: Behavioral studies of the mouse fetus , 1997, Behavioural Processes.

[31]  M. Neville CHAPTER 3 – Determinants of Milk Volume and Composition: A. Lactogenesis in Women: A Cascade of Events Revealed by Milk Composition , 1995 .

[32]  I. Johanson,et al.  Effects of altered olfactory experiences on the development of infant rats' responses to odors. , 1996, Developmental psychobiology.

[33]  P. Hepper Human fetal "olfactory" learning , 1995 .

[34]  W. P. Smotherman,et al.  In utero taste/odor aversion conditioning in the rat , 1982, Physiology & Behavior.

[35]  R. Apfelbach,et al.  Chemical Signals in Vertebrates VII , 1995 .

[36]  H. Prechtl,et al.  The behavioural states of the newborn infant (a review). , 1974, Brain research.

[37]  M. Busnel,et al.  Human Fetal Auditory Perception , 1995 .

[38]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Right-nostril advantage for discrimination of odors , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  J. Winberg,et al.  Does the newborn baby find the nipple by smell? , 1994, The Lancet.

[40]  B. Schaal,et al.  Familiarité et discrimination olfactive chez le nouveau-né : influence différentielle du mode d'alimentation ? , 1997 .

[41]  Helmuth Vorherr,et al.  The Breast: Morphology, Physiology, and Lactation , 1974 .

[42]  D. Hornung,et al.  Olfactory sensitivity: Is there laterality? , 1982 .

[43]  W. P. Smotherman Odor aversion learning by the rat fetus , 1982, Physiology & Behavior.

[44]  R. Sullivan,et al.  Olfactory classical conditioning in neonates. , 1991, Pediatrics.

[45]  R. Soussignan,et al.  Responsiveness to the odour of amniotic fluid in the human neonate. , 1995, Biology of the neonate.

[46]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  Maternal diet alters the sensory qualities of human milk and the nursling's behavior. , 1991, Pediatrics.

[47]  W. W. Read,et al.  Human milk lipids. I. Changes in fatty acid composition of early colostrum. , 1965, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[48]  Mh Teicher,et al.  First suckling response of the newborn albino rat: The role of olfaction and amniotic fluid , 1977 .

[49]  W. Fifer,et al.  The effects of fetal experience with sound. , 1995 .

[50]  B. Schaal,et al.  “Microsmatic Humans” Revisited: The Generation and Perception of Chemical Signals , 1991 .

[51]  B. Schaal Olfaction in infants and children: developmental and functional perspectives , 1988 .

[52]  F. Provenza,et al.  Garlic in the ovine fetal environment , 1992, Physiology & Behavior.

[53]  P. A. Montgomery,et al.  Approaches to the Study of Colostrum-The Onset of Lactation , 1986 .

[54]  G. Beauchamp,et al.  The transfer of alcohol to human milk. Effects on flavor and the infant's behavior. , 1991, The New England journal of medicine.