Following new task instructions: Evidence for a dissociation between knowing and doing
暂无分享,去创建一个
Senne Braem | Marcel Brass | Baptist Liefooghe | Jan De Houwer | M. Brass | S. Braem | J. Houwer | B. Liefooghe
[1] J. de Houwer,et al. Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[2] Michael W. Cole,et al. The power of instructions: Proactive configuration of stimulus-response translation. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] P. Zelazo,et al. An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .
[4] J. Houwer,et al. Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] M Eimer,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al. Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.
[7] J. Duncan,et al. Intelligence and the Frontal Lobe: The Organization of Goal-Directed Behavior , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] Michael W. Cole,et al. Rapid Transfer of Abstract Rules to Novel Contexts in Human Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[9] John Duncan,et al. Goal neglect and knowledge chunking in the construction of novel behaviour☆ , 2014, Cognition.
[10] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[11] John Duncan,et al. Assembly and Use of New Task Rules in Fronto-parietal Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Working memory load but not multitasking eliminates the prepared reflex: further evidence from the adapted flanker paradigm. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[13] Hannes Ruge,et al. Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[14] C. Lewis,et al. When knowledge is not enough: the phenomenon of goal neglect in preschool children. , 2007, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[15] Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience Systems Neuroscience , 2022 .
[16] Philip David Zelazo,et al. Age‐related Asynchrony of Knowledge and Action , 1991 .
[17] Trenton E. Kriete,et al. Generalisation benefits of output gating in a model of prefrontal cortex , 2011, Connect. Sci..
[18] A. Ishai,et al. Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.
[19] Marcel Brass,et al. There is more into ‘doing’ than ‘knowing’: The function of the right inferior frontal sulcus is specific for implementing versus memorising verbal instructions , 2016, NeuroImage.
[20] Christopher H. Chatham,et al. Corticostriatal Output Gating during Selection from Working Memory , 2014, Neuron.
[21] W. Notebaert,et al. Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. , 2016, Psychological bulletin.
[22] J. Duncan,et al. Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions , 1995, Neuropsychologia.
[23] M. Kane,et al. Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[24] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.
[25] M. D’Esposito. Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.
[26] E Berendsen,et al. Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[27] M. Kane,et al. GOAL NEGLECT AND WORKING MEMORY IN PRESS CHILD DEVELOPMENT Goal Neglect and Working Memory Capacity in 4to 6-Year-Old Children , 2009 .
[28] Mike Anderson,et al. Task structure complexity and goal neglect in typically developing children. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[29] Daniel J Mitchell,et al. Neural Coding for Instruction‐Based Task Sets in Human Frontoparietal and Visual Cortex , 2016, Cerebral cortex.
[30] David Badre,et al. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] M. Steinhauser,et al. Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[32] H. Munk. Zur Physiologie der Grosshirnrinde , 1902 .
[33] Andreas Roepstorff,et al. What’s at the top in the top-down control of action? Script-sharing and ‘top-top’ control of action in cognitive experiments , 2004, Psychological research.
[34] Learning through instructions vs. learning through practice: flanker congruency effects from instructed and applied S-R mappings , 2014, Psychological research.
[35] Adele Diamond,et al. Frontal lobe involvement in cognitive changes during the first year of life. , 1991 .
[36] A. Baddeley. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] Baptist Liefooghe,et al. Automatic motor activation by mere instruction , 2014, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.
[38] J. Duncan. The Structure of Cognition: Attentional Episodes in Mind and Brain , 2013, Neuron.
[39] Klus Oberauer,et al. Declarative and Procedural Working Memory: Common Principles, Common Capacity Limits? , 2010 .
[40] M. Brass,et al. It wasn’t me! Motor activation from irrelevant spatial information in the absence of a response , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[41] A. Baddeley,et al. Working memory and binding in sentence recall , 2009 .
[42] Marcel Brass,et al. Cross-talk of instructed and applied arbitrary visuomotor mappings. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[43] P. Zelazo. The development of conscious control in childhood , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] B. Hommel. The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .
[45] Word and deed: a computational model of instruction following , 2011 .
[46] Nachshon Meiran,et al. The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S-R instructions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.
[47] Michael W. Cole,et al. Reflexive activation of newly instructed stimulus–response rules: evidence from lateralized readiness potentials in no-go trials , 2015, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[48] Marcel Brass,et al. The implementation of verbal instructions: Dissociating motor preparation from the formation of stimulus–response associations , 2012, NeuroImage.
[49] M. Brass,et al. The implementation of verbal instructions: An fMRI study , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[50] Stefaan Vandorpe,et al. Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[51] Mark Hallett,et al. Ideomotor apraxia: A review , 2007, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.
[52] Michael W. Cole,et al. When planning results in loss of control: intention-based reflexivity and working-memory , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[53] Marcel Brass,et al. Neural Correlates of Overcoming Interference from Instructed and Implemented Stimulus–Response Associations , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[54] Michael W. Cole,et al. Prefrontal Dynamics Underlying Rapid Instructed Task Learning Reverse with Practice , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[55] M. Brass,et al. There Are Limits to the Effects of Task Instructions: Making the Automatic Effects of Task Instructions Context-Specific Takes Practice , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[56] Adam C. Riggall,et al. The Relationship between Working Memory Storage and Elevated Activity as Measured with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[57] A. Vandierendonck. A Working Memory System With Distributed Executive Control , 2016, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[58] G. Rees,et al. Neuroimaging: Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[59] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[60] B. Milner. Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .
[61] Klaus Oberauer,et al. Design for a working memory. , 2009 .
[62] Nachshon Meiran,et al. The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[63] John Duncan,et al. Goal neglect and Spearman's g: competing parts of a complex task. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[64] A. Whiten,et al. Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.
[65] Y. Miyashita,et al. Functional MRI of Macaque Monkeys Performing a Cognitive Set-Shifting Task , 2002, Science.