Histories of social engagement and adult cognition: midlife in the U.S. study.

OBJECTIVES To evaluate whether social contacts, support, and social strain/conflict are related to executive function and memory abilities in middle-age and older adults. METHODS Longitudinal data on social contacts, support, and strain/conflict were examined in relation to executive function and memory at ages 35-85 years using data from the national Midlife in the U.S. (MIDUS) study. Age-related differences in patterns of association were also examined. RESULTS Regression analyses, controlling for age, sex, race, education, chronic health conditions, and health behaviors, revealed significant positive associations between histories of greater social contacts and support and both executive function and episodic memory, whereas declines in social contacts were negatively associated with both outcomes. Greater average reported frequency of social exchanges characterized by strain or conflict was negatively associated with executive function but not episodic memory. Patterns were generally consistent across different age groups; where differences were seen, associations were stronger in younger age group. DISCUSSION Positive and negative aspects of social relationships are related to cognition throughout adulthood, consistent with the hypothesis that social factors have life-long influences on cognition. Positive and negative aspects of social engagement may thus be important factors to consider in relation to efforts to promote optimal cognitive development and cognitive aging.

[1]  C. McCulloch,et al.  Women Who Maintain Optimal Cognitive Function into Old Age , 2007 .

[2]  M. Lachman,et al.  Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: education, sex, and task complexity matter. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[3]  R. Kessler,et al.  How healthy are we?: A national study of well-being at midlife. , 2004 .

[4]  R. Kessler,et al.  Age and depression in the MIDUS survey: A National Study of Well-Being at Midlife , 2004 .

[5]  L. Berkman,et al.  The Effect of Race and Health-Related Factors on Naming and Memory , 2000, Journal of aging and health.

[6]  M. Hofer Early social relationships: a psychobiologist's view. , 1987, Child development.

[7]  E. Cadenas,et al.  Physiology and Neurobiology of Stress and Adaptation: Central Role of the Brain , 2007 .

[8]  T. Seeman,et al.  Impact of Social Environment Characteristics on Neuroendocrine Regulation , 1996, Psychosomatic medicine.

[9]  T. Seeman,et al.  Social network characteristics and onset of ADL disability: MacArthur studies of successful aging. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[10]  G. Hommel A stagewise rejective multiple test procedure based on a modified Bonferroni test , 1988 .

[11]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Health Effects on Cognitive Aging , 2000 .

[12]  A. Otero,et al.  Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[13]  B. Winblad,et al.  Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study , 2000, The Lancet.

[14]  Y. Stern,et al.  Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s Disease , 2001, Neurology.

[15]  D Commenges,et al.  Marital status and risk of Alzheimer’s disease , 1999, Neurology.

[16]  N. Fox,et al.  The caregiving context in institution-reared and family-reared infants and toddlers in Romania. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  George W Rebok,et al.  Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[18]  S. Goldberg,et al.  Book Review: ATTACHMENT THEORY: SOCIAL, DEVELOPMENTAL, AND CLINICAL PERSPECTIVES. Edited by Susan Goldberg, Roy Muir, and John Kerr. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press, 1995, 520 pp., $49.95 , 1999 .

[19]  J. Jolles,et al.  Engaged lifestyle and cognitive function in middle and old-aged, non-demented persons: a reciprocal association? , 2002, Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie.

[20]  R. Baumeister,et al.  What do men want? Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: comment on Cross and Madson (1997) , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Andreas Seidler,et al.  Association between the psychosocial network and dementia--a case-control study. , 2003, Journal of psychiatric research.

[22]  Patricia A Tun,et al.  Telephone assessment of cognitive function in adulthood: the Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone. , 2006, Age and ageing.

[23]  J W Rowe,et al.  Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  B. Singer,et al.  Social Inequalities in Health and Well-Being: The Role of Relational and Religious Protective Factors. , 2019 .

[25]  S. Syme,et al.  Social support and health. , 1986 .

[26]  V. Menec The relation between everyday activities and successful aging: a 6-year longitudinal study. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[27]  A. Kramer,et al.  Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults , 2003, Psychological science.

[28]  M. Albert,et al.  Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur studies of successful aging. , 2001, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[29]  A. Devries,et al.  Social modulation of stress responses , 2003, Physiology & Behavior.

[30]  T. Seeman,et al.  Protective and Damaging Effects of Mediators of Stress: Elaborating and Testing the Concepts of Allostasis and Allostatic Load , 1999, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[31]  D. Deeg,et al.  Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults. , 2002, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[32]  J. Manfreda,et al.  The effect of age on risk factors for ischemic heart disease: the Manitoba Follow-Up Study, 1948-1993. , 1998, Annals of epidemiology.

[33]  Laura Fratiglioni,et al.  Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[34]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Psychology. Evolution of the social brain. , 2003, Science.

[35]  M. Diamond Response of the brain to enrichment. , 2001, Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciencias.

[36]  Patricia A Tun,et al.  Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. , 2010, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[37]  Susan Goldberg,et al.  Hidden Regulators: Implications for a New Understanding of Attachment, Separation, and Loss , 2013 .

[38]  D. Steffens,et al.  Contribution of Depression to Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Older Adults , 2007, The neurologist.

[39]  C. Ryff,et al.  Who Participates? Accounting for Longitudinal Retention in the MIDUS National Study of Health and Well-Being , 2010, Journal of aging and health.

[40]  R. Kronmal,et al.  Analysing the relationship between change in a risk factor and risk of disease. , 1992, Statistics in medicine.

[41]  S. Syme,et al.  Issues in the study and application of social support. , 1985 .

[42]  T. Strandberg,et al.  Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[43]  François Béland,et al.  Trajectories of cognitive decline and social relations. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[44]  B. Winblad,et al.  An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia , 2004, The Lancet Neurology.

[45]  F. Lung,et al.  Cognitive impairment and associated risk factors among aged community members , 2004, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[46]  R. Hardy,et al.  Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. , 2003, Social science & medicine.

[47]  V. Keith,et al.  Effects of positive and negative social exchanges with various sources on depressive symptoms in younger and older adults. , 1998, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[48]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The relationship between social support and physiological processes: a review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[49]  L. Berkman,et al.  Social Disengagement and Incident Cognitive Decline in Community-Dwelling Elderly Persons , 1999, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[50]  J. Burns,et al.  Psychosis: A costly by-product of social brain evolution in Homo sapiens , 2006, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[51]  R. Dixon,et al.  Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[52]  T. Seeman Social ties and health: the benefits of social integration. , 1996, Annals of epidemiology.

[53]  Thomas A Glass,et al.  Social Engagement and Depressive Symptoms in Late Life , 2006, Journal of aging and health.

[54]  M. Glymour,et al.  Effects of social integration on preserving memory function in a nationally representative US elderly population. , 2008, American journal of public health.

[55]  R. Houts,et al.  Predicting individual differences in attention, memory, and planning in first graders from experiences at home, child care, and school. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[56]  Douglas P Kiel,et al.  Competing Risk of Death: An Important Consideration in Studies of Older Adults , 2010, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[57]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Psychology. Evolution of the social brain. , 2003 .

[58]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Achieving and maintaining cognitive vitality with aging. , 2002, Mayo Clinic proceedings.

[59]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. , 2005, Psychology and aging.

[60]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  The development of the social brain in human infancy , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.