The role of goal relevance in the occurrence of systematic slip errors in routine procedural tasks

Slip errors can have severe consequences but are notoriously difficult to reduce. Training, visual cues and increasing motivation are generally not effective in eliminating these slips. Instead, the approach this work takes is to identify which steps in a routine task are most error prone, so that these can be designed out of device interactions. In particular, device- and task-oriented steps are investigated. Device-oriented steps are "extra" steps imposed by the device that do not directly contribute towards the task goal. Conversely, task-oriented steps directly bring the user closer to their goal. The main hypothesis addressed in this work is that device-oriented steps are more problematic than task-oriented ones. The concepts of device- and task-oriented steps are investigated more closely, by analysing the literature on routine action and mental representations of different steps. The core diff erence between the steps is found to be how relevant a step is to the goal. This is further supported by two qualitative studies. A series of experimental studies investigates the cognitive mechanisms underlying device and task-oriented steps. This is addressed through six experiments that address error rates, step times, proportion of omissions and sensitivity to working memory load. Participants learned one of three routine tasks, with several carefully controlled device- and task-oriented steps. The results show that on device-oriented steps, error rates are higher, step times are longer, the proportion of omissions is greater, and working memory load has an increased effect. These findings support the hypothesis that activation levels are lower on device-oriented steps. The thesis concludes that a step's relevance to the task goal plays an important role in the occurrence of errors. This work has implications for both our understanding of routine procedural action as well as the design of devices.

[1]  M. Gazzaniga The cognitive neurosciences, 3rd edition , 2004 .

[2]  J. Henderson,et al.  The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing , 1999 .

[3]  Jens Rasmussen,et al.  Cognitive Control and Human Error Mechanisms , 1987 .

[4]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Different Cognitive Mechanisms Account for Different Types of Procedural Steps , 2009 .

[5]  D. Ballard,et al.  Eye movements in natural behavior , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Visual Cues to Reduce Errors in a Routine Procedural Task , 2004 .

[7]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Capturing the distinction between task and device errors in a formal model of user behaviour , 2011, Electron. Commun. Eur. Assoc. Softw. Sci. Technol..

[8]  Richard M. Young,et al.  Device-Oriented and Task-Oriented Exploratory Learning of Interactive Devices , 2000 .

[9]  Richard L. Lewis,et al.  The mind and brain of short-term memory. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[10]  Ann Blandford,et al.  The role of communication goals in interaction , 2000 .

[11]  Raj M. Ratwani,et al.  Predicting postcompletion errors using eye movements , 2008, CHI.

[12]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Interruption of the Tower of London task: support for a goal-activation approach. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  Thomas P. Moran,et al.  Getting into a system: External-internal task mapping analysis , 1983, CHI '83.

[14]  Abigail Sellen,et al.  The Prevention of Mode Errors Through Sensory Feedback , 1992, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[15]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Goal Formation and the Unselected Window Problem , 2007 .

[16]  Raj M. Ratwani A spatial memory mechanism for guiding primary task resumption , 2008 .

[17]  Anna Louise Cox,et al.  The Effect of Repeated Cue Exposure on Post-Completion Errors , 2011, CogSci.

[18]  R. Young Surrogates and mappings: two kinds of conceptual models for interactive , 1983 .

[19]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Evaluating Systematic Error Predictions in a Routine Procedural Task , 2007 .

[20]  Andy Whitefield,et al.  On Distinguishing Work Tasks and Enabling Tasks , 1993, Interact. Comput..

[21]  Jonathan Back,et al.  Locked-out: investigating the effectiveness of system lockouts to reduce errors in routine tasks , 2010, CHI Extended Abstracts.

[22]  H. Collewijn,et al.  The function of visual search and memory in sequential looking tasks , 1995, Vision Research.

[23]  George W Rebok,et al.  Pilot error in air carrier mishaps: longitudinal trends among 558 reports, 1983-2002. , 2008, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[24]  E. M. Altmann,et al.  Timecourse of recovery from task interruption: Data and a model , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Preventing Postcompletion Errors: How Much Cue Is Enough? , 2008 .

[26]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  The soft constraints hypothesis: a rational analysis approach to resource allocation for interactive behavior. , 2006, Psychological review.

[27]  Robert S. Bridger,et al.  Effects of dialogue design on automatic teller machine (ATM) usability: Transaction times and card loss , 2000, Behav. Inf. Technol..

[28]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[29]  T. Shallice,et al.  CONTENTION SCHEDULING AND THE CONTROL OF ROUTINE ACTIVITIES , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[30]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Further investigations into post-completion error : the effects of interruption position and duration , 2006 .

[31]  Sheryl L. Miller,et al.  When using the tool interferes with doing the task , 1996, CHI Conference Companion.

[32]  D. Norman Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .

[33]  S. Y.-W. Li An empirical investigation of post-completion error : a cognitive perspective , 2006 .

[34]  Franklin P. Tamborello A Computational Model of Routine Procedural Memory , 2009 .

[35]  Stephen J. Payne,et al.  Acquisition of display-based skill , 1994, CHI Conference Companion.

[36]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Slip errors and cue salience , 2007, ECCE '07.

[37]  Richard M. Young,et al.  Learning Consistent, Interactive, and Meaningful Task-Action Mappings: A Computational Model , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Working Memory Load Affects Device-Specific but Not Task-Specific Error Rates , 2010 .

[39]  C. Spearman,et al.  The Origin of Error , 2022 .

[40]  Matthew M Botvinick,et al.  Empirical and computational support for context-dependent representations of serial order: reply to Bowers, Damian, and Davis (2009). , 2009, Psychological review.

[41]  Ann Blandford,et al.  Does Being Motivated to Avoid Procedural Errors Influence Their Systematicity , 2007 .

[42]  A. Newell Unified Theories of Cognition , 1990 .

[43]  Raj M. Ratwani,et al.  A memory for goals model of sequence errors , 2011, Cognitive Systems Research.

[44]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  Andrew Howes,et al.  The Nature of Device Models: The Yoked State Space Hypothesis and Some Experiments With Text Editors , 1990, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[46]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Task Structure and Postcompletion Error in the Execution of a Routine Procedure , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[47]  G. Vargo The Atomic Chef???and Other True Tales of Design, Error, and Human Technology , 2006 .

[48]  C. Lebiere,et al.  The Atomic Components of Thought , 1998 .

[49]  S. M. Casey,et al.  Set Phasers on Stun: And Other True Tales of Design, Technology, and Human Error , 1998 .

[50]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[51]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[52]  James Reason,et al.  Human Error , 1990 .

[53]  Ann Blandford,et al.  The effect of interruptions on postcompletion and other procedural errors: an account based on the activation-based goal memory model. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[54]  Jens Rasmussen,et al.  Human errors. a taxonomy for describing human malfunction in industrial installations , 1982 .

[55]  Ann Blandford,et al.  The roles of conceptual device models and user goals in avoiding device initialization errors , 2010, Interact. Comput..

[56]  Michael D. Byrne,et al.  Cue effectiveness in mitigating postcompletion errors in a routine procedural task , 2008, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[57]  Emanuel Schmider,et al.  Is It Really Robust , 2010 .

[58]  M. Byrne,et al.  A Working Memory Model of a Common Procedural Error , 1997 .

[59]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  Distraction and action slips in an everyday task: Evidence for a dynamic representation of task context , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[60]  John W. Senders,et al.  Human Error: Cause, Prediction, and Reduction , 1991 .

[61]  Elizabeth F. Churchill,et al.  Modelling Representations of Device Knowledge in Soar , 1991 .

[62]  T. Shallice,et al.  Hierarchical schemas and goals in the control of sequential behavior. , 2006, Psychological review.

[63]  David E. Kieras,et al.  The Role of a Mental Model in Learning to Operate a Device. , 1984 .

[64]  Wai-Tat Fu,et al.  Soft constraints in interactive behavior: the case of ignoring perfect knowledge in-the-world for imperfect knowledge in-the-head , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[65]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[66]  Richard M. Young,et al.  The Machine Inside the Machine: Users' Models of Pocket Calculators , 1981, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..

[67]  Denis Mareschal,et al.  Action selection in complex routinized sequential behaviors. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  Wayne D. Gray The nature and processing of errors in interactive behavior , 2000, Cogn. Sci..

[69]  Raj M. Ratwani,et al.  Developing a Predictive Model of Postcompletion Errors , 2009 .

[70]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[71]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[72]  Andrew F. Monk,et al.  Mode Errors: A User-Centered Analysis and Some Preventative Measures Using Keying-Contingent Sound , 1986, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..

[73]  D. Plaut,et al.  Doing without schema hierarchies: a recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. , 2004, Psychological review.

[74]  D. Plaut,et al.  Representing task context: proposals based on a connectionist model of action , 2002, Psychological research.

[75]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[76]  J. Reason,et al.  Combating omission errors through task analysis and good reminders , 2002, Quality & safety in health care.

[77]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications , 1980 .