When Bad Feels Good: Assistance Failures and Interface Preferences

User interfaces often attempt to assist users by automating elements of interaction, but these attempts will periodically fail -- impeding user performance. To understand the design implications of correct and incorrect assistance, we conducted an experiment in which subjects selected their preferred of two interfaces (neutral and snapping) for a series of 10 drag-and-drop tasks. With neutral the dragged object moved pixel-by-pixel, and with snapping the object snapped to a grid. Snapping trials were engineered to provide controlled levels of objective performance gains and losses with respect to neutral: gains were achieved when the target was aligned with the grid, and losses were achieved through misalignment -- which required subjects to drop the object, hold a key, and complete the task using a finer movement resolution. Results showed a significant preference for the snapping interface, even when losses impaired performance.

[1]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  I.,et al.  Fitts' Law as a Research and Design Tool in Human-Computer Interaction , 1992, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[3]  D. Bernoulli Exposition of a New Theory on the Measurement of Risk , 1954 .

[4]  Youjae Yi A Critical review of consumer satisfaction , 1989 .

[5]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Advances in behavioral economics , 2004 .

[6]  Kasper Hornbæk,et al.  Current practice in measuring usability: Challenges to usability studies and research , 2006, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[7]  Richard P. Larrick,et al.  A prospect theory model of goal behavior , 2008 .

[8]  Kasper Hornbæk,et al.  Measuring usability: are effectiveness, efficiency, and satisfaction really correlated? , 2000, CHI.

[9]  Jakob Nielsen,et al.  Measuring usability: preference vs. performance , 1994, CACM.

[10]  Marc Hassenzahl,et al.  The Interplay of Beauty, Goodness, and Usability in Interactive Products , 2004, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[11]  Ben Shneiderman,et al.  Determining Causes and Severity of End-User Frustration , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Interact..

[12]  G. Peeters,et al.  The positive-negative asymmetry: On cognitive consistency and positivity bias , 1971 .

[13]  Faruk Gul A Theory of Disappointment Aversion , 1991 .

[14]  Clifford Nass,et al.  Emotion in human-computer interaction , 2002 .

[15]  Kasper Hornbæk,et al.  Meta-analysis of correlations among usability measures , 2007, CHI.

[16]  G. Pitz,et al.  Judgment and decision: theory and application. , 1984, Annual review of psychology.

[17]  Joachim Meyer,et al.  Economic and subjective measures of the perceived value of aesthetics and usability , 2006, TCHI.

[18]  H. Simon,et al.  Theories of Decision-Making in Economics and Behavioural Science , 1966 .

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[20]  Carl Gutwin,et al.  Examining the Peak-End Effects of Subjective Experience , 2015, CHI.

[21]  Colin Camerer Bounded Rationality in Individual Decision Making , 1998 .

[22]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[23]  Robert Bell,et al.  Rethinking the progress bar , 2007, UIST.

[24]  R. Thaler Toward a positive theory of consumer choice , 1980 .

[25]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[26]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[27]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Mindful judgment and decision making. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[28]  M. Rabin Psychology and Economics , 1997 .

[29]  James A. Landay,et al.  Utility of human-computer interactions: toward a science of preference measurement , 2011, CHI.

[30]  G. Peeters,et al.  Positive-Negative Asymmetry in Evaluations: The Distinction Between Affective and Informational Negativity Effects , 1990 .

[31]  Shumin Zhai,et al.  A Cost-Benefit Study of Text Entry Suggestion Interaction , 2016, CHI.

[32]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .

[33]  A. K. Basu A Theory of Decision-Making , 1973, The Journal of Sociology & Social Welfare.

[34]  Colin Camerer Individual Decision Making , 2020, The Handbook of Experimental Economics.

[35]  Andrew Howes,et al.  Adaptive Interaction: A Utility Maximization Approach to Understanding Human Interaction with Technology , 2013, Adaptive Interaction: A Utility Maximization Approach to Understanding Human Interaction with Technology.

[36]  Ben Shneiderman,et al.  Severity and impact of computer user frustration: A comparison of student and workplace users , 2006, Interact. Comput..

[37]  D. Kahneman,et al.  When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .

[38]  J. Mercer,et al.  PROSPECT THEORY AND POLITICAL SCIENCE , 2005 .

[39]  E. Berscheid The Pollyanna Principle: Selectivity in Language, Memory, and Thought , 1981 .

[40]  M. Allais Le comportement de l'homme rationnel devant le risque : critique des postulats et axiomes de l'ecole americaine , 1953 .

[41]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion , 2001 .

[42]  R. Oliver A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions , 1980 .

[43]  Rolph E. Anderson Consumer Dissatisfaction: The Effect of Disconfirmed Expectancy on Perceived Product Performance , 1973 .