Response to distress in infants at risk for autism: a prospective longitudinal study.

BACKGROUND Infants and preschoolers with ASD show impairment in their responses to other people's distress relative to children with other developmental delays and typically developing children. This deficit is expected to disrupt social interactions, social learning, and the formation of close relationships. Response to distress has not been evaluated previously in infants with ASD earlier than 18 months of age. METHODS Participants were 103 infant siblings of children with autism and 55 low-risk controls. All children were screened for ASD at 36 months and 14 were diagnosed with ASD. Infants' responsiveness to distress was evaluated at 12, 18, 24, and 36 months. An examiner pretended to hit her finger with a toy mallet and infants' responses were video-recorded. Attention to the examiner and congruent changes in affect were coded on four-point Likert scales. RESULTS Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses confirm that the ASD group paid less attention and demonstrated less change in affect in response to the examiner's distress relative to the high-risk and low-risk participants who were not subsequently diagnosed with ASD. Group differences remained when verbal skills and general social responsiveness were included in the analytic models. CONCLUSIONS Diagnostic groups differ on distress response from 12 to 36 months of age. Distress-response measures are predictive of later ASD diagnosis above and beyond verbal impairments. Distress response is a worthwhile target for early intervention programs.

[1]  A. Rozga,et al.  A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. , 2010, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[2]  D. Segal Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) , 2010 .

[3]  S. Rogers,et al.  Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. , 2009, Developmental science.

[4]  M. Arbib Evolving the language-ready brain and the social mechanisms that support language. , 2009, Journal of communication disorders.

[5]  W. Stone,et al.  Predicting Social Impairment and ASD Diagnosis in Younger Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  Jean Decety,et al.  Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Mark Sheskin,et al.  Visual disengagement in the infant siblings of children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[8]  John C. Mazziotta,et al.  Mirroring others' emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children , 2008, NeuroImage.

[9]  Agata Rozga,et al.  A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[10]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: social engagement, communication, and cognition. , 2006, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[11]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  J. Brian,et al.  Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[15]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  B. Leventhal,et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[17]  G. Baranek Autism During Infancy: A Retrospective Video Analysis of Sensory-Motor and Social Behaviors at 9–12 Months of Age , 1999, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[18]  M. Sigman,et al.  Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioral and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. , 1998, Child development.

[19]  A. Bailey,et al.  Autism: The Phenotype in Relatives , 1998, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  D. Keltner,et al.  Emotion, Social Function, and Psychopathology , 1998 .

[21]  D. Allen,et al.  The Responses of Autistic Children to the Distress of Others , 1998, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[22]  G. Baird,et al.  Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[23]  A. Bailey,et al.  Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study , 1995, Psychological Medicine.

[24]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[25]  G. Dawson,et al.  Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[26]  C. Kasari,et al.  Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. , 1992, Child development.

[27]  P. Mundy,et al.  Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. , 1992, Child development.

[28]  S. Harris,et al.  Perceptions of Child Attachment and Maternal Gratification in Mothers of Children With Autism and Down Syndrome , 1990 .

[29]  C. Gillberg,et al.  A twin study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden. , 1989, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[30]  Megan R. Gunnar,et al.  The effects of maternal positive, neutral and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys , 1987 .

[31]  J. Cohn,et al.  Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated maternal depression. , 1983, Child development.

[32]  L. Carver,et al.  12-month-old infants allocate increased neural resources to stimuli associated with negative adult emotion. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[33]  S. Rogers,et al.  Visual Fixation Patterns during Reciprocal Social Interaction Distinguish a Subgroup of 6-Month-Old Infants At-Risk for Autism from Comparison Infants , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  Jennifer H. Pfeifer,et al.  Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Development of concern for others. , 1992 .

[36]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  5 – The Development of Altruism: Alternative Research Strategies , 1982 .

[37]  R. D. Clark,et al.  Distress crying in neonates: Species and peer specificity. , 1982 .