Theoretical perspectives on language and communication problems in mental retardation and developmental disabilities.

We argue that researchers interested in language and communication problems in mental retardation or any other developmental disorder should view such problems as emerging within the broader context of the behavioral profile, or phenotype, associated with a particular genetic condition. This will require understanding the direct and indirect effects of genes on the development of language and communication and thereby an understanding of the complex relations that exist between language and other dimensions of psychological and behavioral functioning as well as an understanding of the environments in which the developing person acts and is acted upon. We believe that the dominant model for understanding language and communication problems--the nativist approach, which emphasizes the child's innate capacity for acquiring language and characterizes language as consisting of a set of context-free deterministic rules that operate on abstract representations--is inconsistent with an emphasis on indirect genetic effects. We review recent evidence that undermines the nativist approach--evidence concerning the initial state of the language-learning child, the role of environmental input, the competence-performance distinction, and modularity. In place of nativism, we argue for Emergentism, which is a model in which language is seen to emerge from the interaction between the child's biological abilities to map statistical properties of the language input into a distributed representation and the characteristics of the language learning environment and for the purpose of engaging in real-time, meaningful language use.

[1]  J. Werker,et al.  Influences on infant speech processing: toward a new synthesis. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  I. Prigogine,et al.  Exploring Complexity: An Introduction , 1989 .

[3]  V. Marchman,et al.  From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets , 1993, Cognition.

[4]  T. Gelder,et al.  Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition , 1995 .

[5]  Noam Chomsky Reflections on Language. , 1977 .

[6]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Genotype–phenotype relationship in female carriers of the premutation and full mutation of FMR-1 , 1998, Psychiatry Research.

[7]  R. Carlyon,et al.  Auditory temporal processing impairment: neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[8]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[9]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[10]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  Sentence processing strategies in children with expressive and expressive-receptive specific language impairments. , 1999, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[11]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Crawling versus walking infants' perception of affordances for locomotion over sloping surfaces. , 1993, Child development.

[12]  Gary D. Prideaux,et al.  Syntactic form and textual rhetoric: The cognitive basis for certain pragmatic principles , 1991 .

[13]  J. Bohannon,et al.  The issue of negative evidence: Adult responses to children's language errors. , 1988 .

[14]  J. Rondal Exceptional Language Development in Down Syndrome: Implications for the Cognition-Language Relationship , 1995 .

[15]  S. Curtiss Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: Abnormal language acquisition and the modularity of language , 1988 .

[16]  P. Lombroso,et al.  Molecular mechanisms of developmental disorders , 1998, Development and Psychopathology.

[17]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[18]  P. Gordon Learnability and Feedback , 1990 .

[19]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[20]  Judith M. Rumsey,et al.  Neuroimaging : a window to the neurological foundations of learning and behavior in children , 1996 .

[21]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants' memory for spoken words. , 1997, Science.

[22]  R S Chapman,et al.  Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: I. Comprehension. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[23]  N. Smith Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals , 1999 .

[24]  S. Scarr,et al.  Biological and cultural diversity: the legacy of Darwin for development. , 1993, Child development.

[25]  V. Marchman,et al.  Morphological productivity in children with normal language and SLI: a study of the English past tense. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[26]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Language Acquisition and Use: Learning and Applying Probabilistic Constraints , 1997, Science.

[27]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  No negative evidence revisited: Beyond learnability or who has to prove what to whom. , 1990 .

[28]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  Environmental input and cognitive growth: a study using time-period comparisons. , 1998, Child development.

[29]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Language and problems of knowledge : the Managua lectures , 1990 .

[30]  E. Thelen,et al.  Newborn stepping: An explanation for a "disappearing" reflex. , 1982 .

[31]  A. Karmiloff-Smith,et al.  Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. , 1999, Science.

[32]  R. Hodapp Direct and indirect behavioral effects of different genetic disorders of mental retardation. , 1997, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[33]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Language as a dynamical system , 1996 .

[34]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study , 1982, Cognition.

[35]  J. Fodor The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .

[36]  M. Hautzinger,et al.  Fragile-X carrier females: evidence for a distinct psychopathological phenotype? , 1996, American journal of medical genetics.

[37]  Kevin Crowley,et al.  The microgenetic method. A direct means for studying cognitive development. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[38]  Patricia K. Kohl Early linguistic experience and phonetic perception: implications for theories of developmental speech perception , 1993 .

[39]  Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al.  A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[40]  B. Pennington,et al.  The Neurocognitive Phenotype of Female Carriers of Fragile X: Additional Evidence for Specificity , 1993, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[41]  R S Chapman,et al.  Language skills of children and adolescents with Down syndrome: II. Production deficits. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[42]  E. Mark Gold,et al.  Language Identification in the Limit , 1967, Inf. Control..

[43]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[44]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[45]  P. Tallal,et al.  Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[46]  V. Volterra,et al.  A story description task in children with Down's syndrome: lexical and morphosyntactic abilities. , 1997, Journal of intellectual disability research : JIDR.

[47]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  T. Kuhn,et al.  The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. , 1964 .