Changing patterns of eye-head coordination during 6 h of optically reversed vision

Summary1) This study investigates the early development of adaptive changes in oculomotor function associated with coordinated eye-head tracking of the optically reversed image of an earth-fixed target seen through horizontally reversing dove prism goggles attached to the skull. 2) Two tasks comprised a) fixation of a single target during head rotation which causes the seen target's image to move in the direction of head motion by an amount exactly equal to the head movement itself (the 1-Target task), and b) change of gaze onto a displaced target with head free to move (2-Target task). 3) The 1-Target task requires the eyes to move in a direction opposite to that of the normal vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). The 2-Target task is identical, except that reorientation onto the new target calls for an initial saccadic eye movement in a direction opposite to that of the ensuing head movement, which is contrary to the normal pattern of eye-head coordination during gaze shifts. 4) Eye (EOG) and head (potentiometer) movements were continuously recorded (0–250 Hz) in an apparatus which permitted sudden, unexpected, electromagnetic braking of the head movement, either just before or during the intended manœuvre. 5) Early adaptive strategies employed reduction of VOR gain, rearrangement of timing, amplitude and shape of “catch-up” saccades and the introduction of centrally programmed eye movements uncovered by the braking manœuvres. 6) All of these phenomena were detectable in an initial series of 60 trials, in which the total exposure to visual-vestibular conflict was less than 30 s. They became more systematized and more marked after 6 h of active reversed vision experience. 7) Specifically, mean VOR gain, measured within the first 80 ms of head movement (deemed free of visuomotor influence), became markedly attenuated (25% in the first test series; 66% after 6 h of active visionreversed exercise). In addition (not included in the above percentages) there were numerous occasions of complete absence of measurable VOR during head rotation, in both the first and final test series. 8) In the 1-Target task, the latency of the first “catch-up” saccade (re onset of head movement) tended to offset residual VOR by becoming shortened to the point of synchrony with head movement onset. This saccade (not present in control tests) continued to occur on those occasions when the head was unpredictably prevented from moving, and when head movements were made in the dark. 9) Sometimes these initial “saccades” began normally, but “glissaded” in a graded manner into a “smooth pursuit”like trajectory, resembling the classical glissade associated with pulse-step mismatch in the saccade generating system. 10) All these events represent embryo facsimilies of more advanced adaptive manœuvres seen in an earlier study extending over 19 days of reversed vision experience. 11) It is concluded that the adaptive process is a multifactorial one, exhibiting idiosyncracy in individuals and from time to time. Some phenomena appear in embryo form within seconds of exposure to the new condition. Others, such as progressive VOR gain attenuation, introduction of central programming and advanced strategies of the “glissade” type, developed more slowly over the 6 h period of these experiments.

[1]  K. Mori [Electrophysiological experiment on neuronal pathway controlling horizontal eye-head coordination in the cat]. , 1992, Nippon Ganka Gakkai zasshi.

[2]  G. Jones,et al.  Extreme vestibulo‐ocular adaptation induced by prolonged optical reversal of vision , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Adaptive modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex by mental effort in darkness , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control : facts and theories , 1985 .

[6]  D. Guitton,et al.  Stimulation of the superior colliculus in the alert cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  L. Optican,et al.  Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D. A. Robinson,et al.  A model of quick phase generation in the vestibuloocular reflex , 1978, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  D. Robinson Control of eye movements , 1981 .

[10]  F A Miles,et al.  Visually induced adaptive changes in primate saccadic oculomotor control signals. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  G. Barnes Vestibulo‐ocular function during co‐ordinated head and eye movements to acquire visual targets. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  D. Robinson,et al.  Voluntary, non-visual control of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex. , 1976, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[13]  W. Precht,et al.  Lesion-Induced Neuronal Plasticity in Sensorimotor Systems , 1981, Proceedings in Life Sciences.

[14]  B. Fischer Express saccades in man and monkey. , 1986, Progress in brain research.

[15]  B. Peterson,et al.  Voluntary modulation of the vestibuloocular reflex in humans and its relation to smooth pursuit , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Eye-Head Coordination in Monkeys: Evidence for Centrally Patterned Organization , 1971, Science.

[17]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Plasticity in the vestibulo-ocular reflex: a new hypothesis. , 1981, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms underlying eye-head coordination. , 1986, Progress in brain research.

[19]  R M Steinman,et al.  Compensatory eye movements during active and passive head movements: fast adaptation to changes in visual magnification. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  L M Optican,et al.  Adaptive properties of the saccadic system. , 1985, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[21]  R. Tomlinson,et al.  Analysis of human vestibulo-ocular reflex during active head movements. , 1980, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[22]  M. Crommelinck,et al.  Stimulation of the superior colliculus in the alert cat , 1980, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  D. Robinson,et al.  The vestibulo‐ocular reflex during human saccadic eye movements. , 1986, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  Optican Lm Adaptive properties of the saccadic system. , 1985 .

[25]  M. Ito Cerebellar control of the vestibulo-ocular reflex--around the flocculus hypothesis. , 1982, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  R. Douglas,et al.  Eye-head coordination in cats. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Horizontal eye position-related activity in neck muscles of the alert cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  A Berthoz,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in eye-head coordination. , 1985, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[29]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[30]  D. Guitton,et al.  Gaze control in humans: eye-head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  G M Jones,et al.  Plasticity in the adult vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. , 1977, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[32]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Long-Term Effects of Dove Prism Vision on Torsional VOR and Head-Eye Coordination , 1981 .