Visual aperiodic temporal prediction increases perceptual sensitivity and reduces response latencies.

As a predictive organ, the brain can predict upcoming events to guide perception and action in the process of adaptive behavior. The classical models of oscillatory entrainment explain the facilitating effects that occur after periodic stimulation in behavior but cannot explain aperiodic facilitating effects. In the present study, by comparing the behavior performance of participants in periodic predictable (PP), aperiodic predictable (AP) and aperiodic unpredictable (AU) stimulus streams, we investigated the effect of an aperiodic predictable stream on the perceptual sensitivity and response latencies in the visual modality. The results showed that there was no difference between PP and AP conditions in sensitivity (d') and reaction times (RTs), both of which were significantly different from those in the AU condition. Moreover, a significant correlation between d' and RTs was observed when predictability existed. These results indicate that the aperiodic predictable stimulus streams increases perceptual sensitivity and reduces response latencies in a top-down manner. Individuals proactively and flexibly predict upcoming events based on the temporal structure of visual stimuli in the service of adaptive behavior.

[1]  Dance to the rhythm, cautiously: Isolating unique indicators of oscillatory entrainment , 2017, PLoS biology.

[2]  D. Schön,et al.  Rhythm implicitly affects temporal orienting of attention across modalities. , 2013, Acta psychologica.

[3]  Aijun Wang,et al.  How modality processing differences affect cross-modal nonspatial repetition inhibition. , 2020, PsyCh journal.

[4]  Alexandre Gramfort,et al.  Encoding of event timing in the phase of neural oscillations , 2014, NeuroImage.

[5]  D. Senkowski,et al.  The multifaceted interplay between attention and multisensory integration , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Aijun Wang,et al.  Cross-modal nonspatial repetition inhibition: An ERP study , 2020, Neuroscience Letters.

[7]  O. Jensen,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against Anticipated Distracters , 2012, Current Biology.

[8]  Xun He,et al.  A consumer-grade LCD monitor for precise visual stimulation , 2018, Behavior research methods.

[9]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Early processing stages are modulated when auditory stimuli are presented at an attended moment in time: an event-related potential study. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[10]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time. Modulation of brain potentials. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[12]  A. Nobre,et al.  Temporal Expectation Enhances Contrast Sensitivity by Phase Entrainment of Low-Frequency Oscillations in Visual Cortex , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  When What You Hear Influences When You See , 2013, Psychological science.

[15]  J. Obleser,et al.  What do we talk about when we talk about rhythm? , 2017, PLoS biology.

[16]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Finding and Feeling the Musical Beat: Striatal Dissociations between Detection and Prediction of Regularity , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  D. Sanabria,et al.  Temporal Preparation Driven by Rhythms is Resistant to Working Memory Interference , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[18]  Aijun Wang,et al.  Different visual and auditory latencies affect cross-modal non-spatial repetition inhibition. , 2019, Acta psychologica.

[19]  A. Zador,et al.  Auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  S. Haegens,et al.  Rhythmic facilitation of sensory processing: A critical review , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  G. Karmos,et al.  Entrainment of Neuronal Oscillations as a Mechanism of Attentional Selection , 2008, Science.

[22]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Proactive Sensing of Periodic and Aperiodic Auditory Patterns , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[24]  Elyse Sussman,et al.  Auditory Target Detection Is Affected by Implicit Temporal and Spatial Expectations , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  N A Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory analysis of group data: estimating sensitivity from average hit and false-alarm rates. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  Seul Lee,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Visual Attention Allocation , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[27]  M. Jones,et al.  Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays , 2002, Psychological science.

[28]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  M. Jones,et al.  Effects of auditory pattern structure on anticipatory and reactive attending , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  Gregor Thut,et al.  No changes in parieto‐occipital alpha during neural phase locking to visual quasi‐periodic theta‐, alpha‐, and beta‐band stimulation , 2017, bioRxiv.

[31]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  A Corticostriatal Neural System Enhances Auditory Perception through Temporal Context Processing , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Charles E. Schroeder,et al.  Prominence of delta oscillatory rhythms in the motor cortex and their relevance for auditory and speech perception , 2019, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[33]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  A. Kok,et al.  Intermodal spatial attention differs between vision and audition: an event-related potential analysis. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[35]  Luc H. Arnal,et al.  Temporal Prediction in lieu of Periodic Stimulation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Sylvain Baillet,et al.  Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Alejandro Lleras,et al.  Making Waves in the Stream of Consciousness: Entraining Oscillations in EEG Alpha and Fluctuations in Visual Awareness with Rhythmic Visual Stimulation , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  FMRI investigation of cross-modal interactions in beat perception: Audition primes vision, but not vice versa , 2011, NeuroImage.

[39]  Assaf Breska,et al.  When Synchronizing to Rhythms Is Not a Good Thing: Modulations of Preparatory and Post-Target Neural Activity When Shifting Attention Away from On-Beat Times of a Distracting Rhythm , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  B. Postle,et al.  Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Double dissociation of single-interval and rhythmic temporal prediction in cerebellar degeneration and Parkinson’s disease , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  L. Mo,et al.  Dissociable identity‐ and modality‐specific neural representations as revealed by cross‐modal nonspatial inhibition of return , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[43]  L. Deouell,et al.  Neural mechanisms of rhythm-based temporal prediction: Delta phase-locking reflects temporal predictability but not rhythmic entrainment , 2017, PLoS biology.

[44]  Benjamin Morillon,et al.  Natural rhythms of periodic temporal attention , 2020, Nature Communications.

[45]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  Pulsed Out of Awareness: EEG Alpha Oscillations Represent a Pulsed-Inhibition of Ongoing Cortical Processing , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[47]  Floris P. de Lange,et al.  Local Entrainment of Alpha Oscillations by Visual Stimuli Causes Cyclic Modulation of Perception , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Kyle E. Mathewson,et al.  Power and Phase of Alpha Oscillations Reveal an Interaction between Spatial and Temporal Visual Attention , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  David Poeppel,et al.  The Effect of Imagination on Stimulation: The Functional Specificity of Efference Copies in Speech Processing , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  D. Sanabria,et al.  Auditory temporal preparation induced by rhythmic cues during concurrent auditory working memory tasks. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Robin A. A. Ince,et al.  Frontal Top-Down Signals Increase Coupling of Auditory Low-Frequency Oscillations to Continuous Speech in Human Listeners , 2015, Current Biology.

[52]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  To See or Not to See: Prestimulus α Phase Predicts Visual Awareness , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  K. Lange Brain correlates of early auditory processing are attenuated by expectations for time and pitch , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[54]  K. Mathewson,et al.  Rescuing stimuli from invisibility: Inducing a momentary release from visual masking with pre-target entrainment , 2010, Cognition.

[55]  Ulrich Pomper,et al.  Distinct patterns of local oscillatory activity and functional connectivity underlie intersensory attention and temporal prediction , 2016, Cortex.

[56]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A default Bayesian hypothesis test for correlations and partial correlations , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  D. Cheyne,et al.  Cortical dynamics of selective attention to somatosensory events , 2010, NeuroImage.