Effects of task and task-switching on temporal inhibition of return, facilitation of return, and saccadic momentum during scene viewing.

During scene viewing, saccades directed toward a recently fixated location tend to be delayed relative to saccades in other directions ("delay effect"), an effect attributable to inhibition of return (IOR) and/or saccadic momentum (SM). Previous work indicates this effect may be task-specific, suggesting that gaze control parameters are task-relevant and potentially affected by task-switching. Accordingly, the present study investigated task-set control of gaze behavior using the delay effect as a measure of task performance. The delay effect was measured as the effect of relative saccade direction on preceding fixation duration. Participants were cued on each trial to perform either a search, memory, or rating task. Tasks were performed either in pure-task or mixed-task blocks. This design allowed separation of switch-cost and mixing-cost. The critical result was that expression of the delay effect at 2-back locations was reversed on switch versus repeat trials such that return was delayed in repeat trials but speeded in switch trials. This difference between repeat and switch trials suggests that gaze-relevant parameters may be represented and switched as part of a task-set. Existing and new tests for dissociating IOR and SM accounts of the delay effect converged on the conclusion that the delay at 2-back locations was due to SM, and that task-switching affects SM. Additionally, the new test simultaneously replicated noncorroborating results in the literature regarding facilitation-of-return (FOR), which confirmed its existence and showed that FOR is "reversed" SM that occurs when preceding and current saccades are both directed toward the 2-back location.

[1]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  John M. Henderson,et al.  Temporal Oculomotor Inhibition of Return and Spatial Facilitation of Return in a Visual Encoding Task , 2013 .

[3]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  W. Joseph Macinnes,et al.  Inhibition of Return Biases Orienting During the Search of Complex Scenes , 2003, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[5]  J. Pratt,et al.  Examining task difficulty and the time course of inhibition of return: detecting perceptually degraded targets. , 2005, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[6]  N. Meiran,et al.  On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Novelty Is Not Always the Best Policy , 2009, Psychological science.

[8]  R De Jong,et al.  An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .

[9]  D. Alan Allport,et al.  SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .

[10]  M. Land,et al.  The Roles of Vision and Eye Movements in the Control of Activities of Daily Living , 1998, Perception.

[11]  Sebastiaan Mathôt,et al.  PyGaze: An open-source, cross-platform toolbox for minimal-effort programming of eyetracking experiments , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[12]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  J. Pratt,et al.  The Spatial Distribution of Inhibition of Return , 2001, Psychological science.

[14]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  I. Hooge,et al.  Inhibition of saccade return (ISR): spatio-temporal properties of saccade programming , 2000, Vision Research.

[16]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[17]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[18]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Peter König,et al.  Saccadic Momentum and Facilitation of Return Saccades Contribute to an Optimal Foraging Strategy , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[21]  Jay Pratt,et al.  The Time to Detect Targets at Inhibited and Noninhibited Locations : Preliminary Evidence for Attentional Momentum , 2004 .

[22]  A. L. I︠A︡rbus Eye Movements and Vision , 1967 .

[23]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search , 1988, Nature.

[24]  S. Tipper,et al.  On the Strategic Modulation of the Time Course of Facilitation and Inhibition of Return , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? , 2001, Vision Research.

[26]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. , 2000 .

[27]  L. Shaffer CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[29]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[31]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[32]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Examining the influence of task set on eye movements and fixations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[33]  J. Henderson,et al.  Does Oculomotor Inhibition of Return Influence Fixation Probability during Scene Search? This Strategy Would Require a Mechanism for Keeping Track of Visited Locations , 2022 .

[34]  N. Yeung,et al.  Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[36]  Drew H. Abney,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .

[37]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[38]  C. Shea,et al.  Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  Shaffer Lh CHOICE REACTION WITH VARIABLE S-R MAPPING. , 1965 .

[40]  J. Henderson,et al.  Facilitation of return during scene viewing , 2009 .

[41]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of Return is a Foraging Facilitator in Visual Search , 1999 .

[42]  G. Wylie,et al.  Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. , 1999 .

[43]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[44]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[45]  J. Henderson,et al.  The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing , 1999 .

[46]  S. Los,et al.  On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .

[47]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[48]  Michael L. Mack,et al.  Viewing task influences eye movement control during active scene perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[49]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Involuntary retrieval in alphabet-arithmetic tasks: Task-mixing and task-switching costs , 2005, Psychological research.

[50]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search. , 2006, Psychological review.

[51]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Active inhibition and memory promote exploration and search of natural scenes. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[52]  B. A. Conway,et al.  The effects of laforin, malin, Stbd1, and Ptg deficiencies on heart glycogen levels in Pompe disease mouse models , 2015 .

[53]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[54]  J. Henderson,et al.  Looking back at Waldo: oculomotor inhibition of return does not prevent return fixations. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[55]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  Do object refixations during scene viewing indicate rehearsal in visual working memory? , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[56]  I. Hooge,et al.  Inhibition of return is not a foraging facilitator in saccadic search and free viewing , 2005, Vision Research.

[57]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[58]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  D P Munoz,et al.  Influence of previous visual stimulus or saccade on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .