Anxiety-Like Behavioural Inhibition Is Normative under Environmental Threat-Reward Correlations

Behavioural inhibition is a key anxiety-like behaviour in rodents and humans, distinct from avoidance of danger, and reduced by anxiolytic drugs. In some situations, it is not clear how behavioural inhibition minimises harm or maximises benefit for the agent, and can even appear counterproductive. Extant explanations of this phenomenon make use of descriptive models but do not provide a formal assessment of its adaptive value. This hampers a better understanding of the neural computations underlying anxiety behaviour. Here, we analyse a standard rodent anxiety model, the operant conflict test. We harvest Bayesian Decision Theory to show that behavioural inhibition normatively arises as cost-minimising strategy in temporally correlated environments. Importantly, only if behavioural inhibition is aimed at minimising cost, it depends on probability and magnitude of threat. Harnessing a virtual computer game, we test model predictions in four experiments with human participants. Humans exhibit behavioural inhibition with a strong linear dependence on threat probability and magnitude. Strikingly, inhibition occurs before motor execution and depends on the virtual environment, thus likely resulting from a neural optimisation process rather than a pre-programmed mechanism. Individual trait anxiety scores predict behavioural inhibition, underlining the validity of this anxiety model. These findings put anxiety behaviour into the context of cost-minimisation and optimal inference, and may ultimately pave the way towards a mechanistic understanding of the neural computations gone awry in human anxiety disorder.

[1]  Avishek Adhikari,et al.  Single Units in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex with Anxiety-Related Firing Patterns Are Preferentially Influenced by Ventral Hippocampal Activity , 2011, Neuron.

[2]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[4]  A. Graybiel,et al.  A Corticostriatal Path Targeting Striosomes Controls Decision-Making under Conflict , 2015, Cell.

[5]  Eric L. Schwartz,et al.  Computational Neuroscience , 1993, Neuromethods.

[6]  C. Mathys,et al.  Computational approaches to psychiatry , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  S. File,et al.  Validation of open : closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat , 1985, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[8]  D. R. Britton,et al.  A sensitive open field measure of anxiolytic drug activity , 1981, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[9]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  The evolution of decision rules in complex environments , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  A. Holmes,et al.  Animal models of anxiety: an ethological perspective. , 1997, Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas.

[11]  Joshua A. Gordon,et al.  Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear and innate anxiety , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Luigi Acerbi,et al.  Internal Representations of Temporal Statistics and Feedback Calibrate Motor-Sensory Interval Timing , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[13]  J. Gray,et al.  Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  P. Dayan Instrumental vigour in punishment and reward , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  Dominik R. Bach,et al.  Human Hippocampus Arbitrates Approach-Avoidance Conflict , 2014, Current Biology.

[16]  R. Dolan,et al.  The Known Unknowns: Neural Representation of Second-Order Uncertainty, and Ambiguity , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Erratum: Computational psychiatry [Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (2012), 72–80] , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Ventral hippocampal lesions affect anxiety but not spatial learning , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  P. Dayan,et al.  Goals and Habits in the Brain , 2013, Neuron.

[20]  K. Montgomery,et al.  The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory drive. , 1955 .

[21]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Regional dissociations within the hippocampus—memory and anxiety , 2004, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  J. Lisman,et al.  The Theta-Gamma Neural Code , 2013, Neuron.

[23]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Models of place and grid cell firing and theta rhythmicity , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  An improved algorithm for model-based analysis of evoked skin conductance responses☆ , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Computational psychiatry , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Hongkui Zeng,et al.  Differential Control of Learning and Anxiety along the Dorsoventral Axis of the Dentate Gyrus , 2013, Neuron.

[27]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Decision Theory: What "Should" the Nervous System Do? , 2007, Science.

[28]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  S. A. Morrison,et al.  Differential effects of food availability and nest predation risk on avian reproductive strategies , 2013 .

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Time-series analysis for rapid event-related skin conductance responses , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[31]  B. Beer,et al.  A simple and reliable conflict procedure for testing anti-anxiety agents , 2004, Psychopharmacologia.

[32]  Ken-ichi Amemori,et al.  Localized Microstimulation of Primate Pregenual Cingulate Cortex Induces Negative Decision-Making , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  C. Dickman,et al.  Population responses of small mammals to food supply and predators: a global meta-analysis. , 2013, The Journal of animal ecology.

[34]  K. Tye,et al.  Resolving the neural circuits of anxiety , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  D. Bach,et al.  Influences of habitual and situational bodily symptom focusing on stress responses , 2007 .

[36]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Cognitive Tomography Reveals Complex, Task-Independent Mental Representations , 2013, Current Biology.

[37]  Ben Seymour,et al.  Neural Activity Associated with the Passive Prediction of Ambiguity and Risk for Aversive Events , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  F. Graeff,et al.  Pharmacology of human experimental anxiety. , 2003, Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas.

[39]  K. Montgomery,et al.  The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior. , 1955, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modelling event-related skin conductance responses , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[41]  Mihir A. Topiwala,et al.  Synchronized Activity between the Ventral Hippocampus and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Anxiety , 2010, Neuron.

[42]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Sensory Cue Integration , 2011 .

[43]  G. Griebel,et al.  Risk assessment as an evolved threat detection and analysis process , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[44]  N. Burgess,et al.  Brain oscillations and memory , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[45]  D. Bach A cost minimisation and Bayesian inference model predicts startle reflex modulation across species , 2015, Journal of theoretical biology.