How expectations of pain elicited by consciously and unconsciously perceived cues unfold over time

Expectation can shape the perception of pain within a fraction of time, but little is known about how perceived expectation unfolds over time and modulates pain perception. Here, we combine magnetoencephalography (MEG) and machine learning approaches to track the neural dynamics of expectations of pain in healthy participants with both sexes. We found that the expectation of pain, as conditioned by facial cues, can be decoded from MEG as early as 150 ms and up to 1100 ms after cue onset, but decoding expectation elicited by unconsciously perceived cues requires more time and decays faster compared to consciously perceived ones. Also, results from temporal generalization suggest that neural dynamics of decoding cue-based expectation were predominately sustained during cue presentation but transient after cue presentation. Finally, although decoding expectation elicited by consciously perceived cues were based on a series of time-restricted brain regions during cue presentation decoding relied on the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex after cue presentation for both consciously and unconsciously perceived cues. These findings reveal the conscious and unconscious processing of expectation during pain anticipation and may shed light on enhancing clinical care by demonstrating the impact of expectation cues.

[1]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Placebo-Induced Changes in fMRI in the Anticipation and Experience of Pain , 2004, Science.

[2]  M. Fabre-Thorpe,et al.  From face processing to face recognition: Comparing three different processing levels , 2017, Cognition.

[3]  L. Colloca,et al.  Socially induced placebo analgesia: A comparison of a pre‐recorded versus live face‐to‐face observation , 2014, European journal of pain.

[4]  M. Tseng,et al.  Effects of Positive and Negative Expectations on Human Pain Perception Engage Separate But Interrelated and Dependently Regulated Cerebral Mechanisms , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Jian Kong,et al.  Identifying inter-individual differences in pain threshold using brain connectome: a test-retest reproducible study , 2019, NeuroImage.

[6]  F. Benedetti,et al.  Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning , 2009, PAIN®.

[7]  E. Israel,et al.  Active albuterol or placebo, sham acupuncture, or no intervention in asthma. , 2011, The New England journal of medicine.

[8]  Peter Fransson,et al.  A prefrontal non-opioid mechanism in placebo analgesia , 2010, PAIN.

[9]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[10]  J. Zubieta,et al.  Placebo effects on human μ-opioid activity during pain , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Ronald Dubner,et al.  Ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors , 1978, Pain.

[12]  G. D. Iannetti,et al.  Neural indicators of perceptual variability of pain across species , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  M. Boly,et al.  Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  M. Ingvar,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Nonconscious Activation of Conditioned Placebo and Nocebo Responses , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  J. Kong,et al.  Placebo and nocebo effects: an introduction to psychological and biological mechanisms. , 2014, Handbook of experimental pharmacology.

[16]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  How face perception unfolds over time , 2018, Nature Communications.

[17]  M. Ingvar,et al.  Nonconscious activation of placebo and nocebo pain responses , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Stefan Haufe,et al.  On the interpretation of weight vectors of linear models in multivariate neuroimaging , 2014, NeuroImage.

[19]  Ted J. Kaptchuk,et al.  Distinct neural representations of placebo and nocebo effects , 2015, NeuroImage.

[20]  R. Passingham,et al.  Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  K. Davis,et al.  The dynamic pain connectome , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  E. Halgren,et al.  Dynamic Statistical Parametric Mapping Combining fMRI and MEG for High-Resolution Imaging of Cortical Activity , 2000, Neuron.

[23]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of Neural Activity across Cortical Areas Correlates with Conscious Perception , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  K. Davis,et al.  Dynamic pain connectome functional connectivity and oscillations reflect multiple sclerosis pain , 2018, Pain.

[25]  D. Drossman,et al.  Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome , 2008, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[26]  R. Gollub,et al.  Not seeing or feeling is still believing: conscious and non-conscious pain modulation after direct and observational learning , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[27]  Ted J. Kaptchuk,et al.  An fMRI study on the interaction and dissociation between expectation of pain relief and acupuncture treatment , 2009, NeuroImage.

[28]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[29]  R. Dolan,et al.  Subliminal Instrumental Conditioning Demonstrated in the Human Brain , 2008, Neuron.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Distinct cortical codes and temporal dynamics for conscious and unconscious percepts , 2015, eLife.

[31]  Li Hu,et al.  Mesocorticolimbic Pathways Encode Cue-Based Expectancy Effects on Pain , 2019, Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Cees van Leeuwen,et al.  Efficiency of Conscious Access Improves with Coupling of Slow and Fast Neural Oscillations , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  D. Plaut,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of similarity-based neural representations of facial identity , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[35]  R M Leahy,et al.  A sensor-weighted overlapping-sphere head model and exhaustive head model comparison for MEG. , 1999, Physics in medicine and biology.

[36]  S. Taulu,et al.  Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements , 2006, Physics in medicine and biology.

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: the temporal generalization method , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Radoslaw Martin Cichy,et al.  Resolving human object recognition in space and time , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  K. Davis,et al.  Cross-network coupling of neural oscillations in the dynamic pain connectome reflects chronic neuropathic pain in multiple sclerosis , 2020, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[40]  Gian Domenico Iannetti,et al.  Alpha and gamma oscillation amplitudes synergistically predict the perception of forthcoming nociceptive stimuli , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[41]  M. Sigman,et al.  Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  N. Bolger,et al.  Brain Mediators of Predictive Cue Effects on Perceived Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  K. Wiech,et al.  Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  Luana Colloca,et al.  The neural processes of acquiring placebo effects through observation , 2019, NeuroImage.

[46]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  How expectations shape pain , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[47]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. , 2014, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[48]  Chih-Jen Lin,et al.  LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines , 2011, TIST.

[49]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Brief Maintenance of Seen and Unseen Sensory Information , 2016, Neuron.

[50]  Howard L Fields,et al.  How expectations influence pain. , 2018, Pain.

[51]  I. Kirsch,et al.  Efficacy of antidepressants in adults , 2005, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[52]  Yalda Mohsenzadeh,et al.  Decoding the orientation of contrast edges from MEG evoked and induced responses , 2017, NeuroImage.

[53]  Jian Kong,et al.  A neural mechanism of direct and observational conditioning for placebo and nocebo responses , 2019, NeuroImage.

[54]  Jessica Gerber,et al.  Enhancing treatment of osteoarthritis knee pain by boosting expectancy: A functional neuroimaging study , 2018, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[55]  C. Chapman,et al.  Pain measurement: an overview , 1985, Pain.

[56]  L. Leyman,et al.  The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces: A validation study , 2008 .

[57]  T. Kaptchuk,et al.  Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects , 2010, The Lancet.

[58]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Brain Translates Money into Force: A Neuroimaging Study of Subliminal Motivation , 2007, Science.