Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction

The present study employed event-related fMRI and EEG to investigate the biological basis of the cognitive control of behavior. Using a GO/NOGO task optimized to produce response inhibitions, frequent commission errors, and the opportunity for subsequent behavioral correction, we identified distinct cortical areas associated with each of these specific executive processes. Two cortical systems, one involving right prefrontal and parietal areas and the second regions of the cingulate, underlay inhibitory control. The involvement of these two systems was predicated upon the difficulty or urgency of the inhibition and each was employed to different extents by high- and low-absent-minded subjects. Errors were associated with medial activation incorporating the anterior cingulate and pre-SMA while behavioral alteration subsequent to errors was associated with both the anterior cingulate and the left prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, the EEG data demonstrated that successful response inhibition depended upon the timely activation of cortical areas as predicted by race models of response selection. The results highlight how higher cognitive functions responsible for behavioral control can result from the dynamic interplay of distinct cortical systems.

[1]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal–cingulate interactions in action monitoring , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  C. Degueldre,et al.  The Functional Anatomy of Inhibition Processes Investigated with the Hayling Task , 2001, NeuroImage.

[5]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[6]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[7]  J. Jonides,et al.  Inhibition in verbal working memory revealed by brain activation. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  P. Strick,et al.  Cingulate Motor Areas , 1993 .

[9]  S. Dickman,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Functional and Dysfunctional Impulsivity: Personality and Cognitive Correlates , 1990 .

[10]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[11]  K. Shulman,et al.  Disinhibition syndromes, secondary mania and bipolar disorder in old age. , 1997, Journal of affective disorders.

[12]  M. Gabriel,et al.  Neurobiology of Cingulate Cortex and Limbic Thalamus , 1993 .

[13]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  A. Beech,et al.  Reduced cognitive inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1993, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[15]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[16]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[17]  R. Robinson,et al.  Mechanism of disinhibition after brain lesions. , 1997, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease.

[18]  E E Smith,et al.  The neural substrate and temporal dynamics of interference effects in working memory as revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Marcel Brass,et al.  The Inhibition of Imitative Response Tendencies , 2001, NeuroImage.

[20]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Supervisory control of action and thought selection. , 1993 .

[21]  E. Stein,et al.  A parametric manipulation of central executive functioning. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[23]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[24]  P. Burgess,et al.  The ecological validity of tests of executive function , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[25]  Shane M. O’Mara,et al.  Individual differences discriminate event-related potentials but not performance during response inhibition , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Attention: Selection, Awareness, and Control , 1993 .

[27]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  A. Baddeley Exploring the Central Executive , 1996 .

[30]  C Andrew,et al.  Motor response suppression and the prepotent tendency to respond: a parametric fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  M C Ridding,et al.  The effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on suppression of habitual counting during random number generation. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Localization of a Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1994 .

[33]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[34]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Subprocesses of Performance Monitoring: A Dissociation of Error Processing and Response Competition Revealed by Event-Related fMRI and ERPs , 2001, NeuroImage.

[35]  E. Stein,et al.  Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Mario Liotti,et al.  Inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: event-related potentials identify the processing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[37]  R. Chan,et al.  Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: a study with the use of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[38]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[40]  D. Broadbent,et al.  The Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. , 1982, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[41]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[42]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 1997, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[44]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[45]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[46]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Saccadic disinhibition in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.