Common mechanisms of spatial attention in memory and perception: a tactile dual-task study.

Orienting attention to locations in mnemonic representations engages processes that functionally and anatomically overlap the neural circuitry guiding prospective shifts of spatial attention. The attention-based rehearsal account predicts that the requirement to withdraw attention from a memorized location impairs memory accuracy. In a dual-task study, we simultaneously presented retro-cues and pre-cues to guide spatial attention in short-term memory (STM) and perception, respectively. The spatial direction of each cue was independent of the other. The locations indicated by the combined cues could be compatible (same hand) or incompatible (opposite hands). Incompatible directional cues decreased lateralized activity in brain potentials evoked by visual cues, indicating interference in the generation of prospective attention shifts. The detection of external stimuli at the prospectively cued location was impaired when the memorized location was part of the perceptually ignored hand. The disruption of attention-based rehearsal by means of incompatible pre-cues reduced memory accuracy and affected encoding of tactile test stimuli at the retrospectively cued hand. These findings highlight the functional significance of spatial attention for spatial STM. The bidirectional interactions between both tasks demonstrate that spatial attention is a shared neural resource of a capacity-limited system that regulates information processing in internal and external stimulus representations.

[1]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Sustained spatial attention to vibration is mediated in primary somatosensory cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.

[2]  A. Jha,et al.  Tracking the time-course of attentional involvement in spatial working memory: an event-related potential investigation. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  G. R Mangun,et al.  Shifting visual attention in space: an electrophysiological analysis using high spatial resolution mapping , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[4]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[5]  P. Stern Now you feel it, now you don't. , 2016, Science.

[6]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Working memory in primate sensory systems , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  J Debecker,et al.  Wave form and neural mechanism of the decision P350 elicited without pre-stimulus CNV or readiness potential in random sequences of near-threshold auditory clicks and finger stimuli. , 1979, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  Li Hu,et al.  A time-varying source connectivity approach to reveal human somatosensory information processing , 2012, NeuroImage.

[9]  C. Curtis,et al.  Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[11]  T. D. Waberski,et al.  Spatiotemporal Imaging of Electrical Activity Related to Attention to Somatosensory Stimulation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[12]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[13]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting Attention to Locations in Internal Representations , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Steven Lemm,et al.  Now you feel it--now you don't: ERP correlates of somatosensory awareness. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[15]  F. Smulders,et al.  Lateralized ERP components related to spatial orienting: discriminating the direction of attention from processing sensory aspects of the cue. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[16]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  A. Nobre,et al.  Cognitive control of attention in the human brain: Insights from orienting attention to mental representations , 2006, Brain Research.

[18]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[19]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Visual and spatial working memory: From boxes to networks , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Timing and spatial distribution of somatosensory responses recorded in the upper bank of the sylvian fissure (SII area) in humans. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  J. Jonides,et al.  Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Interactions between working memory, attention and eye movements. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[26]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Covert attention in touch: behavioral and ERP evidence for costs and benefits. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[27]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Effects of hand posture on preparatory control processes and sensory modulations in tactile-spatial attention , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[28]  M. Eimer,et al.  Modulations of early somatosensory ERP components by transient and sustained spatial attention , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  A. Nobre,et al.  Markers of preparatory attention predict visual short-term memory performance , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  E. Vogel,et al.  Interactions between attention and working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[31]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Attentional modulation of the human somatosensory evoked potential in a trial-by-trial spatial cueing and sustained spatial attention task measured with high density 128 channels EEG. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[32]  R. Knight,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: Contributions of human brain lesion studies , 2006, Neuroscience.

[33]  C. Shagass,et al.  Somatosensory evoked potential changes with a selective attention task. , 1982, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  No functional role of attention-based rehearsal in maintenance of spatial working memory representations. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[35]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Common fronto-parietal activity in attention, memory, and consciousness: Shared demands on integration? , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.

[36]  Søren K. Andersen,et al.  Maintenance of tactile short-term memory for locations is mediated by spatial attention , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[37]  Bettina Forster,et al.  ERP correlates of tactile spatial attention differ under intra- and intermodal conditions , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[38]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[39]  Roy Luria,et al.  Orienting attention to objects in visual short-term memory , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  D. Kourtis,et al.  An early parietal ERP component of the frontoparietal system: EDAN≠N2pc , 2010, Brain Research.

[41]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Søren K. Andersen,et al.  Effects of Feature-selective and Spatial Attention at Different Stages of Visual Processing , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  A. Baddeley Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  M. Eimer,et al.  Do ERP components triggered during attentional orienting represent supramodal attentional control? , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[45]  Edward Awh,et al.  The Role of Spatial Selective Attention in Working Memory for Locations: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Inhibition of saccadic eye movements to locations in spatial working memory , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[47]  H Johansen-Berg,et al.  The physiology and psychology of selective attention to touch. , 2000, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[48]  Bettina Forster,et al.  Sustained Spatial Attention in Touch: Modality-Specific and Multimodal Mechanisms , 2011, TheScientificWorldJournal.

[49]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Confidence intervals in repeated-measures designs: The number of observations principle. , 2009, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[50]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Anterior and posterior attentional control systems use different spatial reference frames: ERP evidence from covert tactile-spatial orienting. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[51]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The relationship between human long-latency somatosensory evoked potentials recorded from the cortical surface and from the scalp. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[53]  Jöran Lepsien,et al.  Searching for Targets within the Spatial Layout of Visual Short-Term Memory , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Crossmodal links in spatial attention are mediated by supramodal control processes: evidence from event-related potentials. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[55]  Martin Eimer,et al.  The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes , 2004, Biological Psychology.

[56]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Somatosensory responses during selective spatial attention: The N120‐to‐N140 trasition , 1995 .

[57]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis , 2007, NeuroImage.

[58]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[59]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Selection within visual memory representations activates the oculomotor system , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[60]  A. Mognon,et al.  ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[61]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[62]  Søren K. Andersen,et al.  Nonspatial Cueing of Tactile STM Causes Shift of Spatial Attention , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[63]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Temporal dynamics of lateralized ERP components elicited during endogenous attentional shifts to relevant tactile events. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[64]  G. Mangun,et al.  Individual working memory capacity is uniquely correlated with feature-based attention when combined with spatial attention , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[65]  Fred Tam,et al.  Functional MRI of working memory and selective attention in vibrotactile frequency discrimination , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[66]  A. Gazzaley Influence of early attentional modulation on working memory , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[67]  Jon Driver,et al.  Effects of attentional filtering demands on preparatory ERPs elicited in a spatial cueing task , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[68]  P. Michie,et al.  The effects of spatial selective attention on the somatosensory event-related potential. , 1987, Psychophysiology.