Sounds of the neighborhood: False memories and the structure of the phonological lexicon

The development of a well-formulated view of the memory storage systems (lexicons) involved in word recognition is a central goal of research on language processes. Assumptions about the organizing characteristics and structures of these memory systems are found in various discussions of lexical neighborhoods (Coltheart, Davelaar, Jonasson, & Besner, 1977) or cohorts (Johnson & Pugh, 1994; Marslen-Wilson, 1990). The focus of neighborhood research in visual word recognition has been primarily at the orthographic level. Several articles have discussed how orthographic neighborhood effects provide insight into the manner by which visual words are translated into sound and meaning during reading. In this article, we move the investigation to the phonological lexicon in an attempt to establish the word characteristics that best reflect phonological lexical organization. We describe two phonological false memory experiments that demonstrate that the initial two phonemes of phonological CVC words play a central role in predicting false memories for unpresented items. We also provide evidence of sustained and complementary activation when lists of items provide converging information about the unpresented critical lure.

[1]  J. Deese On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  B. Underwood FALSE RECOGNITION PRODUCED BY IMPLICIT VERBAL RESPONSES. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  The "Tip of the Tongue" Phenomenon , 1966 .

[4]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[5]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[6]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[7]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .

[8]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Surface dyslexia. , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[9]  D. Bouwhuis,et al.  Attention and performance X : control of language processes , 1986 .

[10]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Function and process in spoken word recognition: A tutorial review , 1984 .

[11]  Ronald A. Cole,et al.  Sound and spelling in spoken word recognition. , 1985 .

[12]  S. Kinoshita,et al.  Visual and Auditory Recognition of Prefixed Words , 1986 .

[13]  W. Marslen-Wilson Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[14]  H. Nusbaum,et al.  Phonological priming in auditory word recognition. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[15]  Helen E. Moss,et al.  Distance and competition in lexical access , 1988 .

[16]  S. Goldinger,et al.  Priming Lexical Neighbors of Spoken Words: Effects of Competition and Inhibition. , 1989, Journal of memory and language.

[17]  Gerry Altmann,et al.  Cognitive Models of Speech Processing: Psycholinguistic and Computational Perspectives - Workshop Overview , 1989, AI Mag..

[18]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access , 1991 .

[19]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[20]  M. T. Reinitz,et al.  Memory-conjunction errors: Miscombination of stored stimulus features can produce illusions of memory , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[21]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Testing a semistochastic variant of the interactive activation model in different word recognition experiments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  M L Stanback,et al.  Syllable and rime patterns for teaching reading: Analysis of a frequency-based vocabulary of 17,602 words , 1992, Annals of dyslexia.

[23]  S. Goldinger,et al.  Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: the roles of competition and bias. , 1992, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[24]  Dawn G. Blasko,et al.  Do the Beginnings of Spoken Words Have a Special Status in Auditory Word Recognition , 1993 .

[25]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[26]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[27]  W Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Levels of perceptual representation and process in lexical access: words, phonemes, and features. , 1994, Psychological review.

[28]  N. F. Johnson,et al.  A Cohort Model of Visual Word Recognition , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  S. Blumstein,et al.  The effect of subphonetic differences on lexical access , 1994, Cognition.

[30]  William P. Wallace,et al.  Recognition Memory Errors Produced by Implicit Activation of Word Candidates during the Processing of Spoken Words , 1995 .

[31]  Yasushi Hino,et al.  Neighborhood Size and Neighborhood Frequency Effects in Word Recognition , 1995 .

[32]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Effects of category length and strength on familiarity in recognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  K. McDermott,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .

[34]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  W. Wallace,et al.  False positives in recognition memory produced by cohort activation , 1995, Cognition.

[36]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[37]  J Grainger,et al.  List context effects on masked phonological priming in the lexical decision task , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. , 1996 .

[40]  J. Read,et al.  From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 minutes: Confusing real and illusory events , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  S. Lupker,et al.  Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .

[42]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the `tip-of-the-tongue' phenomenon , 1997, Cognition.

[43]  D. Schacter,et al.  Illusory memories in amnesic patients: conceptual and perceptual false recognition. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[44]  S. Andrews The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts , 1997 .

[45]  A Caramazza,et al.  Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-the-tongue states. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  M. Carreiras,et al.  Effects of Syllable Frequency and Syllable Neighborhood Frequency in Visual Word Recognition , 1998 .

[47]  S Monsell,et al.  Competitor priming in spoken word recognition. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  W P Wallace,et al.  Are false recognitions influenced by prerecognition processing? , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  S. Gerhand,et al.  Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. , 1998 .

[50]  Conrad Perry,et al.  No more problems in Coltheart's neighborhood: resolving neighborhood conflicts in the lexical decision task , 1998, Cognition.

[51]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phonological Priming by Masked Nonword Primes in the Lexical Decision Task , 1998 .

[52]  N. Brown,et al.  False Memories and Semantic Lexicon Arrangement , 1999, Brain and Language.

[53]  Mitchell S. Sommers,et al.  Who Really Lives Next Door: Creating False Memories with Phonological Neighbors☆☆☆★ , 1999 .

[54]  Yasushi Hino,et al.  Orthographic neighbourhood effects in parallel distributed processing models. , 1999 .

[55]  S. Lupker,et al.  Orthographic neighborhood effects in perceptual identification and semantic categorization tasks: A test of the multiple read-out model , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[56]  D. Balota,et al.  Word frequency, repetition, and lexicality effects in word recognition tasks: beyond measures of central tendency. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  S. Stoness,et al.  Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1999 .

[58]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Estimating the frequency of nonevents: The role of recollection failure in false recognition , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[59]  W. Marslen-Wilson Accessing Spoken Words : The Importance of Word Onsets , 2004 .

[60]  James S. Magnuson,et al.  Spoken Word Recognition in the Visual World Paradigm Reflects the Structure of the Entire Lexicon , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.