Limited Transfer of Newly Acquired Movement Patterns across Walking and Running in Humans

The two major modes of locomotion in humans, walking and running, may be regarded as a function of different speed (walking as slower and running as faster). Recent results using motor learning tasks in humans, as well as more direct evidence from animal models, advocate for independence in the neural control mechanisms underlying different locomotion tasks. In the current study, we investigated the possible independence of the neural mechanisms underlying human walking and running. Subjects were tested on a split-belt treadmill and adapted to walking or running on an asymmetrically driven treadmill surface. Despite the acquisition of asymmetrical movement patterns in the respective modes, the emergence of asymmetrical movement patterns in the subsequent trials was evident only within the same modes (walking after learning to walk and running after learning to run) and only partial in the opposite modes (walking after learning to run and running after learning to walk) (thus transferred only limitedly across the modes). Further, the storage of the acquired movement pattern in each mode was maintained independently of the opposite mode. Combined, these results provide indirect evidence for independence in the neural control mechanisms underlying the two locomotive modes.

[1]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  B. Dobkin,et al.  Can the mammalian lumbar spinal cord learn a motor task? , 1994, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[3]  W. B. Lindquist,et al.  Continuous shifts in the active set of spinal interneurons during changes in locomotor speed , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  D. Yanagihara,et al.  Nitric oxide plays a key role in adaptive control of locomotion in cat. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  Zhiwei Luo,et al.  A mathematical model of adaptive behavior in quadruped locomotion , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[6]  A. Bastian Learning to predict the future: the cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  Shik Ml,et al.  Control of walking and running by means of electric stimulation of the midbrain , 1966 .

[8]  Masaya Hirashima,et al.  Asymmetric Transfer of Visuomotor Learning between Discrete and Rhythmic Movements , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Modular Control of Limb Movements during Human Locomotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Claire Wolstenholme,et al.  How do infants adapt to loading of the limb during the swing phase of stepping? , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  W. T. Thach Correlation of neural discharge with pattern and force of muscular activity, joint position, and direction of intended next movement in motor cortex and cerebellum. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Shik Ml,et al.  Control of walking and running by means of electrical stimulation of the mesencephalon. , 1969 .

[13]  J. Cabelguen,et al.  Bimodal Locomotion Elicited by Electrical Stimulation of the Midbrain in the Salamander Notophthalmus viridescens , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  S. M. Morton,et al.  Cerebellar Contributions to Locomotor Adaptations during Splitbelt Treadmill Walking , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Kram,et al.  Changing the demand on specific muscle groups affects the walk–run transition speed , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[16]  Stephen H Scott,et al.  Limited transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual skills within the same limb , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  D. O'Malley,et al.  Locomotor repertoire of the larval zebrafish: swimming, turning and prey capture. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.

[18]  C. T. Farley,et al.  Energetics of walking and running: insights from simulated reduced-gravity experiments. , 1992, Journal of applied physiology.

[19]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Motor patterns in human walking and running. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Julia T. Choi,et al.  Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  Richard R Neptune,et al.  Differences in muscle function during walking and running at the same speed. , 2006, Journal of biomechanics.

[22]  Daniel P. Ferris,et al.  Effect of reduced gravity on the preferred walk-run transition speed. , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[23]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  A. Berkowitz,et al.  Activity of descending propriospinal axons in the turtle hindlimb enlargement during two forms of fictive scratching: phase analyses , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Split-belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  B I Prilutsky,et al.  Swing- and support-related muscle actions differentially trigger human walk-run and run-walk transitions. , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.