Timing and velocity randomization similarly affect anticipatory pursuit.

Smooth pursuit eye movements are guided largely by retinal-image motion. To compensate for neural conduction delays, the brain employs a predictive mechanism to generate anticipatory pursuit that precedes target motion (E. Kowler, 1990). A critical question for interpreting neural signals recorded during pursuit concerns how this mechanism is interfaced with sensorimotor processing. It has been shown that the predictor is not simply turned-off during randomization because anticipatory eye velocity remains when target velocity is randomized (E. Kowler & S. McKee, 1987; G. W. Kao & M. J. Morrow, 1994). This study was completed to compare pursuit behavior during randomized motion-onset timing with that occurring during direction or speed randomization. We found that anticipatory eye velocity persisted despite motion-onset randomization, and that anticipation onset time was between that observed in the different constant-timing conditions. This centering strategy was similar to the bias of eye velocity magnitude away from extremes observed when direction or speed was randomized. Such a strategy is comparable to least-squares error minimization, and could be used to facilitate acquisition of a target when it begins to move. Centering was in some observers accounted for by a shift of eye velocity toward that generated in the preceding trial. The results make unlikely a model in which the predictor is disengaged by randomizing stimulus timing, and suggest that predictive signals always interact with those used in sensorimotor processing during smooth pursuit.

[1]  G. R. Barnes,et al.  The remembered pursuit task: evidence for segregation of timing and velocity storage in predictive oculomotor control , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Human smooth pursuit direction discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[3]  Heinen,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(97)00422-7 , 1998 .

[4]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Smooth-pursuit initiation in the presence of a textured background in monkey , 1986, Vision Research.

[5]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[6]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—I. Periodic target steps , 1979, Vision Research.

[7]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Cortical visual motion processing for oculomotor control. , 1990, Research publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.

[8]  S. G. Lisberger,et al.  A Control Systems Model of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements with Realistic Emergent Properties , 1989, Neural Computation.

[9]  C R Genovese,et al.  Cortical networks subserving pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans: An FMRI study , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[10]  G. Westheimer Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. , 1954, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[11]  D S Zee,et al.  Effects of lesions of the oculomotor cerebellar vermis on eye movements in primate: smooth pursuit. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  G. Barnes,et al.  The influence of display characteristics on active pursuit and passively induced eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  G. Barnes,et al.  Pursuit of intermittently illuminated moving targets in the human. , 1992, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  D. Munoz,et al.  t Immediate Neural Plasticity Shapes Motor Performance , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  R J Krauzlis,et al.  Effects of directional expectations on motion perception and pursuit eye movements , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  Eileen Kowler Cognitive expectations, not habits, control anticipatory smooth oculomotor pursuit , 1989, Vision Research.

[18]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Humans and Monkeys Canceling of Pursuit and Saccadic Eye Movements in , 2003 .

[19]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Predictive Control of Eye Tracking Movements , 1962 .

[20]  S. Heinen Single neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Eileen Kowler The role of visual and cognitive processes in the control of eye movement. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[22]  P. Dallos,et al.  Learning behavior of the eye fixation control system , 1963 .

[23]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—II. Single target displacements , 1979, Vision Research.

[24]  T. Pasternak,et al.  The multiple roles of visual cortical areas MT/MST in remembering the direction of visual motion. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J V Haxby,et al.  Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Generation of smooth-pursuit eye movements: neuronal mechanisms and pathways , 1991, Neuroscience Research.

[28]  M. Liu,et al.  Single-neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth-pursuit eye movements to predictable target motion , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[29]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.

[30]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[31]  J. Haxby,et al.  Functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements in humans as revealed by fMRI. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades , 2000, Neuroreport.

[33]  J. C. Fox,et al.  OPTIC NYSTAGMUS: III. CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SLOW PHASE , 1930 .

[34]  L. Mannucci,et al.  Chemical Stimulation of Synaptosomes Modulates α-Ca2+/Calmodulin-Dependent Protein Kinase II mRNA Association to Polysomes , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Morrow,et al.  The relationship of anticipatory smooth eye movement to smooth pursuit initiation , 1994, Vision Research.

[36]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[37]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[38]  Annette M. Schmid,et al.  An fMRI study of anticipation and learning of smooth pursuit eye movements in humans , 2001, Neuroreport.

[39]  E. Keller,et al.  Characterization of prediction in the primate visual smooth pursuit system. , 1995, Bio Systems.

[40]  G. Barnes,et al.  The mechanism of prediction in human smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[41]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Visual motion processing for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements in humans. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  A. Terry Bahill,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements in response to predictable target motions , 1983, Vision Research.

[43]  J. L. Gordon,et al.  A model of the smooth pursuit eye movement system , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[44]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Postsaccadic enhancement of initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.