The ACTIVE conceptual framework as a structural equation model

ABSTRACT Background/Study Context: Conceptual frameworks are analytic models at a high level of abstraction. Their operationalization can inform randomized trial design and sample size considerations. Methods: The Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) conceptual framework was empirically tested using structural equation modeling (N=2,802). ACTIVE was guided by a conceptual framework for cognitive training in which proximal cognitive abilities (memory, inductive reasoning, speed of processing) mediate treatment-related improvement in primary outcomes (everyday problem-solving, difficulty with activities of daily living, everyday speed, driving difficulty), which in turn lead to improved secondary outcomes (health-related quality of life, health service utilization, mobility). Measurement models for each proximal, primary, and secondary outcome were developed and tested using baseline data. Each construct was then combined in one model to evaluate fit (RMSEA, CFI, normalized residuals of each indicator). To expand the conceptual model and potentially inform future trials, evidence of modification of structural model parameters was evaluated by age, years of education, sex, race, and self-rated health status. Results: Preconceived measurement models for memory, reasoning, speed of processing, everyday problem-solving, instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) difficulty, everyday speed, driving difficulty, and health-related quality of life each fit well to the data (all RMSEA < .05; all CFI > .95). Fit of the full model was excellent (RMSEA = .038; CFI = .924). In contrast with previous findings from ACTIVE regarding who benefits from training, interaction testing revealed associations between proximal abilities and primary outcomes are stronger on average by nonwhite race, worse health, older age, and less education (p < .005). Conclusions: Empirical data confirm the hypothesized ACTIVE conceptual model. Findings suggest that the types of people who show intervention effects on cognitive performance potentially may be different from those with the greatest chance of transfer to real-world activities.

[1]  Cynthia Owsley,et al.  The Life Space Questionnaire: A Measure of the Extent of Mobility of Older Adults , 1999 .

[2]  A. Gross,et al.  Memory training and strategy use in older adults: results from the ACTIVE study. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[3]  H. Harman,et al.  Kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests , 1976 .

[4]  M. Crowe,et al.  Changes in Everyday Function in Individuals with Psychometrically Defined Mild Cognitive Impairment in the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Study , 2007, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[5]  S. Willis,et al.  Dimensionality of everyday problem solving in older adults. , 1995, Psychology and aging.

[6]  John J. Prindle,et al.  A latent change score analysis of a randomized clinical trial in reasoning training. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[7]  CynthiaOwsley,et al.  Older Drivers and Cataract : Driving Habits and Crash Risk , 2022 .

[8]  S. Hofer,et al.  Are Changes in Self-Rated Health Associated With Memory Decline in Older Adults? , 2017, Journal of aging and health.

[9]  J. Brandt,et al.  Word list memory predicts everyday function and problem-solving in the elderly: Results from the ACTIVE cognitive intervention trial , 2011, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[10]  Karlene K. Ball,et al.  Speed of Processing Training in the ACTIVE Study , 2013, Journal of aging and health.

[11]  V L KOZLOWSKI,et al.  Depressive symptoms. , 1959, Medical times.

[12]  George W Rebok,et al.  Cognitive predictors of everyday functioning in older adults: results from the ACTIVE Cognitive Intervention Trial. , 2011, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[13]  J. Brandt The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: Development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. , 1991 .

[14]  Dev Sarmah,et al.  Changes in mobility , 2017 .

[15]  Dimensionality of Everyday Problem Solving in Older Adults , 1995 .

[16]  Andreas Ritter,et al.  Structural Equations With Latent Variables , 2016 .

[17]  K. Anstey,et al.  Cross-sectional and longitudinal patterns of dedifferentiation in late-life cognitive and sensory function: the effects of age, ability, attrition, and occasion of measurement. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  George W Rebok,et al.  Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. , 2002, JAMA.

[19]  Martin T. Pietrucha,et al.  Mobility and transportation in the elderly , 2000 .

[20]  M. V. Vander Weg,et al.  Speed of processing training protects self-rated health in older adults: enduring effects observed in the multi-site ACTIVE randomized controlled trial , 2009, International Psychogeriatrics.

[21]  J N Morris,et al.  ACTIVE: a cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults. , 2001, Controlled clinical trials.

[22]  Michael Marsiske,et al.  The Revised Observed Tasks of Daily Living: A Performance-Based Assessment of Everyday Problem Solving in Older Adults , 2005, Journal of applied gerontology : the official journal of the Southern Gerontological Society.

[23]  S. Willis,et al.  Everyday cognitive competence in elderly persons: conceptual issues and empirical findings. , 1996, The Gerontologist.

[24]  Virginia G Wadley,et al.  Changes in mobility among older adults with psychometrically defined mild cognitive impairment. , 2010, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[25]  P. Rubé,et al.  L’examen Clinique en Psychologie , 1959 .

[26]  H. Kraemer,et al.  Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  M. Kurtz Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test , 2017 .

[28]  C. Sherbourne,et al.  The MOS 36-Item Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) , 1992 .

[29]  S. Willis,et al.  Reasoning Training in the ACTIVE Study , 2013, Journal of aging and health.

[30]  K. Ball,et al.  Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. , 2006, JAMA.

[31]  W W Hauck,et al.  A randomized, controlled trial of a home environmental intervention: effect on efficacy and upset in caregivers and on daily function of persons with dementia. , 2001, The Gerontologist.

[32]  Beth T. Stalvey,et al.  Older drivers and cataract: driving habits and crash risk. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[33]  Jeanine M. Parisi,et al.  Parallel but not equivalent: Challenges and solutions for repeated assessment of cognition over time , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[34]  George W Rebok,et al.  Ten‐Year Effects of the Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly Cognitive Training Trial on Cognition and Everyday Functioning in Older Adults , 2014, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[35]  Lesley A Ross,et al.  Examination of cognitive and instrumental functional performance as indicators for driving cessation risk across 3 years. , 2008, The Gerontologist.

[36]  Jeanine M. Parisi,et al.  Memory Training in the ACTIVE Study , 2013, Journal of aging and health.

[37]  D. Roth,et al.  Driving cessation and health trajectories in older adults. , 2009, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[38]  C. McHorney,et al.  The MOS 36‐Item Short‐Form Health Survey (SF‐36): II. Psychometric and Clinical Tests of Validity in Measuring Physical and Mental Health Constructs , 1993, Medical care.

[39]  Cynthia Owsley,et al.  Timed Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Tasks: Relationship to Cognitive Function and Everyday Performance Assessments in Older Adults , 2002, Gerontology.

[40]  Fredric D Wolinsky,et al.  The effects of the ACTIVE cognitive training trial on clinically relevant declines in health-related quality of life. , 2006, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[41]  A M Jette,et al.  The disablement process. , 1994, Social science & medicine.

[42]  P. Bentler,et al.  Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives , 1999 .

[43]  Richard N. Jones,et al.  Depressive symptoms impair everyday problem-solving ability through cognitive abilities in late life. , 2011, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.