Communicating the Consequences of Early Detection: The Role of Evidence and Framing

Despite the enormous benefits of early-detection products, consumers are reluctant to use them. The authors explore this reluctance, testing alternative approaches to communicating the consequences of detection behaviors. The results suggest that anecdotal messages are more involving than statistical messages and that positive anecdotes (about gains from screening) are less persuasive than negative anecdotes (about the losses from failing to get screened); positive anecdotes appear to cause a “boomerang” effect. The authors discuss implications for promoting consumer risk-reduction behaviors.

[1]  Robert L. Mason,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 2003 .

[2]  L. Spencer,et al.  Qualitative data analysis for applied policy research , 2002 .

[3]  K. McPherson,et al.  Cancer patients' information needs and information seeking behaviour: in depth interview study , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[4]  W. Bickel,et al.  Reframing Health Behavior Change With Behavioral Economics , 2000 .

[5]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Temporal changes in the value of objects of choice: Discounting, behavior patterns, and health behavior. , 2000 .

[6]  G. Madden A Behavioral-Economics Primer , 2000 .

[7]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Mindset, optimistic bias about personal risk and health-protective behaviour , 1999 .

[8]  M. H. Maskay,et al.  You're never too old: a cancer education and risk reduction program for the elderly. , 1999, Journal of cancer education : the official journal of the American Association for Cancer Education.

[9]  J. Frost,et al.  Women's interest in vaginal microbicides. , 1999, Family planning perspectives.

[10]  Michael S. LaTour,et al.  There are Threats and (Maybe) Fear-Caused Arousal: Theory and Confusions of Appeals to Fear and Fear Arousal Itself , 1997 .

[11]  Kathryn C. Oleson,et al.  When exceptions prove the rule: how extremity of deviance determines the impact of deviant examples on stereotypes. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  B. Leupen,et al.  Design and analysis , 1997 .

[13]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  K. Mccaul,et al.  Does Concern About Breast Cancer Inhibit or Promote Breast Cancer Screening , 1996 .

[15]  J. Roberts Breast cancer deaths decline in US , 1996, BMJ.

[16]  T. Groves Government plan for care of elderly people , 1996, BMJ.

[17]  Robin L. Snipes,et al.  Don't be afraid to use fear appeals: an experimental study , 1996 .

[18]  G I Balch,et al.  Strategic questions for consumer-based health communications. , 1995, Public health reports.

[19]  Lauren G. Block,et al.  When to Accentuate the Negative: The Effects of Perceived Efficacy and Message Framing on Intentions to Perform a Health-Related Behavior , 1995 .

[20]  S. Trafton Curing the Crisis: Options for America's Health Care . Michael D. Reagan, Boulder, CO:Westview Press,1992, 196 pp. US$58.00 cloth. ISBN 0-8133-8179-7. US$16.95 paper. ISBN 0-8133-8180-0. Westview Press, 5500 Central Ave., Boulder, CO 80301-2847, USA. , 1995, Politics and the Life Sciences.

[21]  James Price Dillard,et al.  Fear appeals in health promotion campaigns: Too much, too little, or just right? , 1995 .

[22]  J. Monahan Thinking positively: Using positive affect when designing health messages. , 1995 .

[23]  Alexander J. Rothman,et al.  The effects of message framing on mammography utilization. , 1995, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[24]  R. Staelin,et al.  A Model of Perceived Risk and Intended Risk-handling Activity , 1994 .

[25]  Hans-Bernd Brosius,et al.  The Utility of Exemplars in Persuasive Communications , 1994 .

[26]  John P. Elder,et al.  Motivating Health Behavior , 1993 .

[27]  Peter Salovey,et al.  The Influence of Message Framing on Intentions to Perform Health Behaviors , 1993 .

[28]  D. Morisky,et al.  Mammography Usage and the Health Belief Model , 1992, Health education quarterly.

[29]  R. Mcdermott,et al.  Breast cancer beliefs of women participating in a television-promoted mammography screening project. , 1992, Public health reports.

[30]  Everett M. Rogers,et al.  Designing Health Communication Campaigns: What Works? , 1992 .

[31]  J. Simpson,et al.  The Concise Oxford dictionary of proverbs , 1992 .

[32]  Deborah H. Gruenfeld,et al.  Semantics and pragmatics of social influence: how affirmations and denials affect beliefs in referent propositions. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  R. Petty,et al.  Expectations of reassurance influence the nature of fear-stimulated attitude change , 1992 .

[34]  V. Jackson,et al.  Self-referred mammography patients: analysis of patients' characteristics. , 1991, AJR. American journal of roentgenology.

[35]  Joan Meyers-Levy,et al.  The Influence of Message Framing and Issue Involvement , 1990 .

[36]  D. Lauver,et al.  Message framing, dispositional optimism, and follow-up for abnormal Papanicolaou tests. , 1990, Research in nursing & health.

[37]  Ali H. Mokdad,et al.  Behavioral risk factor surveillance system. , 1989, Iowa medicine : journal of the Iowa Medical Society.

[38]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The effect of message framing on breast self-examination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  Melvin Manis,et al.  Can the availability heuristic explain vividness effects , 1986 .

[40]  M. Dignan,et al.  Evaluation of the North Carolina Risk Reduction Program for smoking and alcohol. , 1985, The Journal of school health.

[41]  L. James,et al.  Mediators, Moderators, and Tests for Mediation. , 1984 .

[42]  David E. Kanouse,et al.  Explaining Negativity Biases in Evaluation and Choice Behavior: Theory and Research , 1984 .

[43]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effectiveness: The Moderating Role of Involvement , 1983 .

[44]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Stalking the elusive "vividness" effect. , 1982 .

[45]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[46]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .

[47]  R. Hogarth Judgement and choice: The psychology of decision , 1982 .

[48]  A. Isen,et al.  Helping as a function of feeling state and nature of the helping behavior , 1979 .

[49]  L. McNally Semantics and Pragmatics , 1978, Language in Society.

[50]  A. Isen,et al.  The effect of feeling good on a helping task that is incompatible with good mood , 1978 .

[51]  Barton A. Weitz,et al.  Time Horizon Effects on Product Evaluation Strategies , 1977 .

[52]  Barton A. Weitz,et al.  Time horizon effects on product evaluation strategies. , 1977 .

[53]  Melvin L. Snyder,et al.  Prior Exercise of Freedom and Reactance. , 1976 .

[54]  J. Elashoff,et al.  Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research. , 1974 .

[55]  Seymour Geisser,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1963 .

[56]  M. Williams A life in your hands. , 1954, Modern hospital.

[57]  William Allen,et al.  Effects of fear-arousing communications , 1953 .

[58]  Robert K. Merton,et al.  The Focused Interview , 1946, American Journal of Sociology.

[59]  M. Sherif,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1946, Psychological review.