Stone Tools and the Evolution of Human Cognition

Stone tools are the most durable and common type of archaeological remain and one of the most important sources of information about behaviors of early hominins. This book develops methods for examining questions of cognition, demonstrating the progression of mental capabilities from early hominins to modern humans through the archaeological record. Dating as far back as 2.5-2.7 million years ago, stone tools were used in cutting up animals, woodworking, and preparing vegetable matter. Today, lithic remains give archaeologists insight into the forethought, planning, and enhanced working memory of our early ancestors. Contributors focus on multiple ways in which archaeologists can investigate the relationship between tools and the evolving human mind - including joint attention, pattern recognition, memory usage, and the emergence of language. Offering a wide range of approaches and diversity of place and time, the chapters address issues such as skill, social learning, technique, language, and cognition based on lithic technology. The book will be of interest to Paleolithic archaeologists and paleoanthropologists interested in stone tool technology and cognitive evolution.

[1]  D. Grayson The establishment of human antiquity , 1983 .

[2]  C. Boesch,et al.  4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Marina Mosquera Martinez,et al.  Differential Raw Material Use in the Middle Pleistocene of Spain: Evidence from Sierra de Atapuerca, Torralba, Ambrona and Aridos , 1998, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[4]  Dietrich Stout,et al.  The social and cultural context of stone knapping skill acquisition , 2005 .

[5]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking. , 2008, Journal of human evolution.

[6]  S. Placzek All in a Day's Work , 2001 .

[7]  F. Knowles Stone-worker's progress : a study of stone implements in the Pitt Rivers Museum , 1954 .

[8]  Miriam T. Stark,et al.  Alternative Procedures for Assessing Standardization in Ceramic Assemblages , 1996, American Antiquity.

[9]  J. Yellen,et al.  Projectile Technologies of the African MSA , 2006 .

[10]  S. Wurz The Howiesons Poort Backed Artefacts from Klasies River: An Argument for Symbolic Behaviour , 1999 .

[11]  T. Wynn The intelligence of Oldowan hominids , 1981 .

[12]  Shannon P. McPherron,et al.  Handaxes as a Measure of the Mental Capabilities of Early Hominids , 2000 .

[13]  W. G. Smith Man, The Primeval Savage: His Haunts And Relics From The Hill-tops Of Bedfordshire To Blackwall , 2011 .

[14]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  The Middle Pleistocene Lithic Assemblage from Yarimburgaz Cave, Turkey. , 1996 .

[15]  George C. Frison,et al.  A Functional Analysis of Certain Chipped Stone Tools , 1968, American Antiquity.

[16]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  A demographic model for Palaeolithic technological evolution: The case of East Asia and the Movius Line , 2010 .

[17]  T. Givon Toward a neurology of grammar , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  L. Wadley,et al.  A Segment is not a Monolith: evidence from the Howiesons Poort of Sibudu, South Africa , 2008 .

[19]  Mark J. White,et al.  On the Significance of Acheulean Biface Variability in Southern Britain , 1998, Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society.

[20]  Adam Powell,et al.  Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior , 2009, Science.

[21]  Michael J. Rogers,et al.  Cutmarked bones from Pliocene archaeological sites at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia: implications for the function of the world's oldest stone tools. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[22]  D. Stout Skill and Cognition in Stone Tool Production , 2002, Current Anthropology.

[23]  Metin I. Eren,et al.  Experimental Evaluation of the Levallois “Core Shape Maintenance” Hypothesis , 2009 .

[24]  Dietrich Stout,et al.  Raw material selectivity of the earliest stone toolmakers at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[25]  R. Byrne The Evolution of Thought: The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use , 2004 .

[26]  D. Kaufman,et al.  Measuring Archaeological Diversity: An Application of the Jackknife Technique , 1998, American Antiquity.

[27]  A. Jelinek The Tabun Cave and Paleolithic Man in the Levant , 1982, Science.

[28]  Mark W. Moore 2. Lithic design space modelling and cognition in Homo floresiensis , 2007 .

[29]  H Mewhinney A Skeptic Views the Billet Flake , 1964, American Antiquity.

[30]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  Blank Form and Reduction as Determinants of Mousterian Scraper Morphology , 1992, American Antiquity.

[31]  N. Uomini,et al.  Humans, tools and handedness , 2005 .

[32]  N. Toth,et al.  Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya , 1981, Nature.

[33]  S. McPherron,et al.  What typology can tell us about Acheulian handaxe production , 2006 .

[34]  Michael Stafford,et al.  The parallel-flaked flint daggers of late Neolithic Denmark: an experimental perspective , 2003 .

[35]  N. Goren-Inbar,et al.  The Acheulean massive scrapers of Gesher Benot Ya'aqov-a product of the biface chaîne opératoire. , 2008, Journal of human evolution.

[36]  B. Trigger,et al.  World Prehistory: A New Outline. , 1969 .

[37]  T. Wynn,et al.  The role of working memory in the evolution of managed foraging , 2003 .

[38]  G. Clark,et al.  In Search of the Neanderthals: Some Conceptual Issues with Special Reference to the Levant , 1995, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[39]  Jacques Pelegrin,et al.  A framework for analysing prehistoric stone tool manufacture and a tentative application to some early stone industries , 1993 .

[40]  Rw Byrne The maker not the tool: The cognitive significance of great ape manual skills , 2005 .

[41]  T. Wynn The Intelligence of Later Acheulean Hominids , 1979 .

[42]  E. Saltzman,et al.  The development of rulebound strategies for manipulating seriated cups: A parallel between action and grammar , 1972 .

[43]  H. Dibble Middle paleolithic scraper reduction: Background, clarification, and review of the evidence to date , 1995 .

[44]  P. Villa,et al.  The Still Bay points of Blombos Cave (South Africa) , 2009 .

[45]  Christophe Boesch,et al.  Excavation of a Chimpanzee Stone Tool Site in the African Rainforest , 2002, Science.

[46]  Z. Harris,et al.  Foundations of language , 1941 .

[47]  Philip G. Chase Symbols and paleolithic artifacts: Style, standardization, and the imposition of arbitrary form , 1991 .

[48]  V. Roux,et al.  Planning abilities as a dynamic perceptual-motor skill: an actualist study of different levels of expertise involved in stone knapping , 2005 .

[49]  Robert L. Bettinger,et al.  Techniques for Assessing Standardization in Artifact Assemblages: Can We Scale Material Variability? , 2001, American Antiquity.

[50]  J. McNabb The shape of things to come. A speculative essay on the role of the Victoria West phenomenon at Canteen Koppie during the South African earlier stone age , 2001 .

[51]  E. Callahan Experiments with Danish mesolithic microblade technology , 1985 .

[52]  T. Wynn The Evolution of Spatial Competence , 1989 .

[53]  S. Gould,et al.  The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[54]  Mark B. Roberts,et al.  Fairweather Eden: Life Half a Million Years Ago as Revealed by the Excavations at Boxgrove , 1997 .

[55]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.

[56]  Éric Boëda,et al.  Approche de la variabilité des systèmes de production lithique des industries du paléolithique inférieur et moyen : chronique d'une variabilité attendue , 1992 .

[57]  Thomas Wynn,et al.  An Ape's View of the Oldowan , 1989 .

[58]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Sequential learning in non-human primates , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  N. Toth The oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts , 1985 .

[60]  P. Texier,et al.  Isenya: état des recherches sur un nouveau site acheuléen d'Afrique orientale , 1988 .

[61]  C. Karlin,et al.  The ancient mind: Prehistoric technology: a cognitive science? , 1994 .

[62]  A. Brooks,et al.  The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. , 2000, Journal of human evolution.

[63]  K. Gibson,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: New perspectives on instincts and intelligence: Brain size and the emergence of hierarchical mental constructional skills , 1990 .

[64]  Julie C. Stout,et al.  Stone Tool-Making and Brain Activation: Position Emission Tomography (PET) Studies , 2000 .

[65]  S. Lycett Are Victoria West cores "proto-Levallois"? A phylogenetic assessment. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.

[66]  J. Pelegrin,et al.  Remarks about archaeological techniques and methods of knapping : elements of a cognitive approach to stone knapping , 2005 .

[67]  Mark W. Moore,et al.  Australian Aboriginal Blade Production Methods on the Georgina River, Camooweal, Queensland , 2003 .

[68]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  The Big Deal about Blades: Laminar Technologies and Human Evolution , 1999 .

[69]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Does everybody do it? Hierarchically organized sequential activity in robots, birds and monkeys , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[70]  P. Mellars The Neanderthal Legacy , 1995 .

[71]  P. Mellars Major Issues in the Emergence of Modern Humans , 1989, Current Anthropology.

[72]  Lewis R. Binford,et al.  An Archaeological Perspective , 1972 .

[73]  Iain Davidson,et al.  Tools and language in human evolution , 1993 .

[74]  S. Ambrose Chronology of the Later Stone Age and Food Production in East Africa , 1998 .

[75]  Blandine Bril,et al.  General Introduction: a Dynamic Systems Framework for Studying a Uniquely Hominin Innovation , 2007 .

[76]  S. Shennan Demography and Cultural Innovation: a Model and its Implications for the Emergence of Modern Human Culture , 2001, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[77]  P. Kelterborn Towards Replicating Egyptian Predynastic Flint Knives , 1984 .

[78]  S. Parker,et al.  Darwin's Legacy: Scenarios in Human Evolution , 2008 .

[79]  S. Ambrose Paleolithic Technology and Human Evolution , 2001, Science.

[80]  M. Corballis From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[81]  Christopher A Bergman,et al.  Experimental studies in the determination of flaking mode , 1982 .

[82]  H. Dibble,et al.  The Effect of Hammer Mass and Velocity on Flake Mass , 1995 .

[83]  Dennis M. Sandgathe,et al.  Alternative Interpretation of the Levallois Reduction Technique , 2004 .

[84]  H. Dibble The Interpretation of Middle Paleolithic Scraper Morphology , 1987, American Antiquity.

[85]  Jacques Pelegrin,et al.  Long blade technology in the Old World : an experimental approach and some archaeological results , 2006 .

[86]  Barbara J. Roth,et al.  Notched Tool Reuse and Raw Material Availability in French Middle Paleolithic Sites , 1996, American Antiquity.

[87]  P. V. Peer,et al.  The Levallois Reduction Strategy , 1992 .

[88]  T. Chaminade,et al.  Neural correlates of Early Stone Age toolmaking: technology, language and cognition in human evolution , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[89]  Jack Harris,et al.  Primate archaeology , 2009, Nature.

[90]  B. Cotterell,et al.  The Formation of Flakes , 1987, American Antiquity.

[91]  Frederick L Coolidge,et al.  The expert Neandertal mind. , 2004, Journal of human evolution.

[92]  G. Dietrich,et al.  Stone knapping : Khambhat (India), a unique opportunity ? , 2005 .

[93]  S. Kuhn Upper Paleolithic raw material economies at Üçağızlı cave, Turkey , 2004 .

[94]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Toward a neurology of grammar , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[95]  Brian Hayden,et al.  Specialization in the Paleolithic , 1988 .

[96]  Nathan Schlanger Understanding Levallois: Lithic Technology and Cognitive Archaeology , 1996, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[97]  N. Toth Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the lower and middle pleistocene, and its possible implications , 1985 .

[98]  William Andrefsky,et al.  The Analysis of Stone Tool Procurement, Production, and Maintenance , 2009 .

[99]  M. Moore,et al.  The Tula Adze: manufacture and purpose , 2004, Antiquity.

[100]  Van SchyndelZoë Part and Parcel , 2005 .

[101]  C. Boesch,et al.  Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking , 1984, Primates.

[102]  H. Roche,et al.  Early hominid stone tool production and technical skill 2.34 Myr ago in West Turkana, Kenya , 1999, Nature.

[103]  Philip Lieberman,et al.  Toward an Evolutionary Biology of Language , 2006 .

[104]  Shannon J. P. McPherron A reduction model for variability in Acheulian biface morphology , 1994 .

[105]  Mark H. Newcomer,et al.  Some quantitative experiments in handaxe manufacture , 1971 .

[106]  Francis Wenban-Smith,et al.  The use of canonical variates for determination of biface manufacturing technology at boxgrove lower palaeolithic site and the behavioural implications of this technology , 1989 .

[107]  Sophie A. de Beaune,et al.  The Invention of Technology. , 2004 .

[108]  B. Bradley,et al.  Analysis by replication of two Acheulian artefact assemblages , 1986 .

[109]  P. Greenfield,et al.  “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus : Processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language , 1990 .

[110]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Strategies used to combine seriated cups by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), and capuchins (Cebus apella). , 1999, Journal of comparative psychology.

[111]  F. Bordes The Old Stone Age. , 1968 .

[112]  J. Gowlett Mental abilities of early man , 1984 .

[113]  John McNabb,et al.  The Large Cutting Tools from the South African Acheulean and the Question of Social Traditions1 , 2004, Current Anthropology.

[114]  M. Peresani Discoid Lithic Technology , 2003 .

[115]  R. Jackendoff Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution , 2002 .

[116]  J. Speth Mechanical Basis of Percussion Flaking , 1972, American Antiquity.

[117]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[118]  John D Speth,et al.  Experimental investigation of hard-hammer percussion flaking , 1974 .

[119]  J. Pelegrin,et al.  Prehistoric Lithic Technology : Some Aspects of Research , 1990 .

[120]  Andrew W. Pelcin The Effect of Indentor Type on Flake Attributes: Evidence from a Controlled Experiment , 1997 .

[121]  A. Jelinek THE LOWER PALEOLITHIC: CURRENT EVIDENCE AND INTERPRETATIONS , 1977 .

[122]  Nathan Schlanger The ancient mind: Mindful technology: unleashing the chaîne opératoire for an archaeology of mind , 1994 .

[123]  T. Wynn Piaget, stone tools and the evolution of human intelligence. , 1985, World archaeology.

[124]  S. Lycett Why is there a lack of Mode 3 Levallois technologies in East Asia? A phylogenetic test of the Movius-Schick hypothesis , 2007 .

[125]  K. Connolly,et al.  Hierarchies and tool-using strategies , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[126]  Adam Brumm,et al.  Stone artifacts and hominins in island Southeast Asia: new insights from Flores, eastern Indonesia. , 2007, Journal of human evolution.

[127]  Jatmiko,et al.  Continuities in stone flaking technology at Liang Bua, Flores, Indonesia. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.

[128]  G. Monnier Testing Retouched Flake Tool Standardization During the Middle Paleolithic: Patterns and Implications: evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic , 2006 .

[129]  O. Bar‐Yosef,et al.  Definition and interpretation of Levallois technology , 2022 .

[130]  R. Gould,et al.  The Lithic Assemblage of the Western Desert Aborigines of Australia , 1971, American Antiquity.

[131]  I. Davidson The archaeology of language origins–a review , 1991, Antiquity.

[132]  M. White,et al.  Lower Palaeolithic Core Technology and the Origins of the Levallois Method in North‐Western Europe , 2003, Current Anthropology.

[133]  R. Holloway, Culture: A Human Domain , 1969, Current Anthropology.

[134]  Harold L. Dibble,et al.  The Missing Mousterian , 2006, Current Anthropology.

[135]  J. Reyss,et al.  TL Dates of Burnt Flints from Jelinek's Excavations at Tabun and their Implications , 1995 .

[136]  T. Goebel,et al.  Ice Age Atlantis? Exploring the Solutrean-Clovis ‘connection’ , 2005 .

[137]  Bernard Wood,et al.  Older than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use , 2003 .

[138]  Anne Delagnes,et al.  Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills: the case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. , 2005, Journal of human evolution.

[139]  O. Bar‐Yosef,et al.  The Chaîne Opératoire Approach in Middle Paleolithic Archaeology , 2009, Current Anthropology.

[140]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Building a Tree Structure: The Development of Hierarchical Complexity and Interrupted Strategies in Children's Construction Activity. , 1977 .

[141]  F. d’Errico,et al.  Evidence of termite foraging by Swartkrans early hominids. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[142]  B. Bradley,et al.  The Solutrean-Clovis connection: reply to Straus, Meltzer and Goebel , 2006 .

[143]  P. Mellars Cognitive Changes and the Emergence of Modern Humans in Europe , 1991, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[144]  L. Fisher Blades and microliths: Changing contexts of tool production from Magdalenian to Early Mesolithic in southern Germany , 2006 .

[145]  Robson Bonnichsen,et al.  Understanding Stone Tools: A Cognitive Approach , 1984 .

[146]  N. Goren-Inbar,et al.  Cognition and communication in the Levantine Lower Palaeolithic , 1994 .

[147]  N. Goren-Inbar,et al.  THE TECHNOLOGICAL ABILITIES OF THE LEVANTINE MOUSTERIANS Cultural and Mental Capacities , 1998 .

[148]  Dietrich Stout Oldowan toolmaking and hominin brain evolution: theory and research using Positron Emission Tomography (PET) , 2006 .

[149]  W. Noble,et al.  Human Evolution, Language and Mind: A Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry , 1996 .

[150]  Steven L. Kuhn,et al.  "Unpacking" Reduction: Lithic Raw Material Economy in the Mousterian of West-Central Italy , 1991 .

[151]  J. Napier,et al.  A New Species of The Genus Homo From Olduvai Gorge , 1964, Nature.

[152]  T. Wynn Archaeology and cognitive evolution. , 2002, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[153]  H. Breuil,et al.  The Stone Age of Mount Carmel: , 1938, Nature.

[154]  Adam Brumm,et al.  Symbolic Revolutions and the Australian Archaeological Record , 2005, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[155]  W. Farrand,et al.  History of the sedimentary infilling of Yarimburgaz Cave, Turkey , 1997 .

[156]  D. Crabtree Mesoamerican Polyhedral Cores and Prismatic Blades , 1968, American Antiquity.

[157]  John C. Whittaker,et al.  Flintknapping: Making and Understanding Stone Tools , 1994 .

[158]  Peter Hiscock,et al.  Transformations of Upper Palaeolithic implements in the Dabba industry from Haua Fteah (Libya) , 1996, Antiquity.

[159]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[160]  G. Sharon,et al.  Acheulian Giant‐Core Technology , 2009, Current Anthropology.

[161]  J. Shea,et al.  The origins of lithic projectile point technology: evidence from Africa, the Levant, and Europe , 2006 .

[162]  Gary T. Garufi,et al.  Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1999 .

[163]  Naama Goren-Inbar,et al.  An Actualistic Study of Dorsally Plain Flakes: A Technological Note , 2001 .

[164]  Stephen J. Lycett,et al.  Acheulean variation and selection: does handaxe symmetry fit neutral expectations? , 2008 .