Contrast of hand preferences between communicative gestures and non-communicative actions in baboons: Implications for the origins of hemispheric specialization for language

Gestural communication is a modality considered in the literature as a candidate for determining the ancestral prerequisites of the emergence of human language. As reported in captive chimpanzees and human children, a study in captive baboons revealed that a communicative gesture elicits stronger degree of right-hand bias than non-communicative actions. It remains unclear if it is the communicative nature of this manual behavior which induces such patterns of handedness. In the present study, we have measured hand use for two uninvestigated behaviors in a group of captive olive baboons: (1) a non-communicative self-touching behavior ("muzzle wipe" serving as a control behavior), (2) another communicative gesture (a ritualized "food beg") different from the one previously studied in the literature (a species-specific threat gesture, namely "hand slap") in the same population of baboons. The hand preferences for the "food beg" gestures revealed a trend toward right-handedness and significantly correlated with the hand preferences previously reported in the hand slap gesture within the same baboons. By contrast, the hand preferences for the self-touching behaviors did not reveal any trend of manual bias at a group-level nor correlation with the hand preferences of any communicative gestures. These findings provide additional support to the hypothesized existence in baboons of a specific communicative system involved in the production of communicative gestures that may tend to a left-hemispheric dominance and that may differ from the system involved in purely motor functions. The hypothetical implications of these collective results are discussed within the theoretical framework about the origins of hemispheric specialization for human language.

[1]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Hand use and gestural communication in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1998, Journal of comparative psychology.

[2]  U. Jürgens Neural pathways underlying vocal control , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  U. Bellugi,et al.  Hand dominance for signing: Clues to brain lateralization of language , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  A. Harcourt The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of Behavior, Jane Goodall. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachussets (1986), xii, +671. Price $30 , 1988 .

[5]  Adam Kendon,et al.  Some Considerations for a Theory of Language Origins , 1991 .

[6]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Understanding the Point of Chimpanzee Pointing , 2005, Current directions in psychological science.

[7]  D. Moody,et al.  Neural lateralization of species-specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). , 1978, Science.

[8]  W. Hopkins,et al.  Gestural communication in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): The influence of experimenter position on gesture type and hand preference , 2002, Laterality.

[9]  M. Annett Left, right, hand and brain : the right shift theory , 1985 .

[10]  W. Hopkins On the Other Hand: Statistical Issues in the Assessment and Interpretation of Hand Preference Data in Nonhuman Primates , 2004, International Journal of Primatology.

[11]  S. Dimond,et al.  Face touching in monkeys, apes and man: Evolutionary origins and cerebral asymmetry , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  William D Hopkins,et al.  Handedness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) is associated with asymmetries of the primary motor cortex but not with homologous language areas. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication , 1993 .

[14]  Eva M. Neumann-Held,et al.  Learning from Animals? : Examining the Nature of Human Uniqueness , 2008 .

[15]  J. M. Warren,et al.  Handedness and laterality in humans and other animals , 1980 .

[16]  W. Hopkins The evolution of hemispheric specialization in primates , 2007 .

[17]  Jacques Vauclair,et al.  Baboons communicate with their right hand , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  G. Michel,et al.  A meta-analysis of primate hand preferences, particularly for reaching. , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[19]  P. Greenfield From hand to mouth , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  J. King,et al.  Laterality in spontaneous motor activity of chimpanzees and squirrel monkeys , 1992, American journal of primatology.

[21]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): Repertoire and use , 2005, American journal of primatology.

[22]  J. Vauclair,et al.  Hand preferences for unimanual and coordinated bimanual tasks in baboons (Papio anubis). , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  C. Teufel,et al.  Lack of orienting asymmetries in Barbary macaques: implications for studies of lateralized auditory processing , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  G. Hewes Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language , 1992, Current Anthropology.

[25]  Allen R. Braun,et al.  Language Lateralization in a Bimanual Language , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  H. Kummer,et al.  Social Organization Of Hamadryas Baboons , 1968 .

[27]  F. D. Waal,et al.  Tree of origin : what primate behavior can tell us about human social evolution , 2001 .

[28]  M. Tomasello CHAPTER 15 – Do Apes Ape? , 1996 .

[29]  A. Whiten,et al.  How do apes ape? , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[30]  Tracy T. Dooley,et al.  Early Sign Language Acquisition and the Development of Hand Preference in Young Children , 1997, Brain and Language.

[31]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech. , 1999 .

[32]  Maurizio Gentilucci,et al.  Speech and gesture share the same communication system , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  A. Braun,et al.  Toward an evolutionary perspective on conceptual representation: species-specific calls activate visual and affective processing systems in the macaque. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  D. McNeill Hand and Mind , 1995 .

[35]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language , 2002 .

[36]  M T McGuire,et al.  Nonverbal interaction of patients and therapists during psychiatric interviews. , 1982, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[37]  Jamie L. Russell,et al.  Communicative Signaling Activates ‘Broca's’ Homolog in Chimpanzees , 2008, Current Biology.

[38]  L. Rogers,et al.  Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  W. McGrew,et al.  On the Other Hand: Current Issues in and Meta-Analysis of the Behavioral Laterality of Hand Function in Nonhuman Primates , 1997 .

[40]  J. Taglialatela Functional and Structural Asymmetries for Auditory Perception and Vocal Production in Nonhuman Primates , 2007 .

[41]  William D. Hopkins,et al.  From hand to mouth in the evolution of language: the influence of vocal behavior on lateralized hand use in manual gestures by chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) , 2003 .

[42]  H. Heffner,et al.  Temporal lobe lesions and perception of species-specific vocalizations by macaques. , 1984, Science.

[43]  M. Hauser,et al.  Left hemisphere dominance for processing vocalizations in adult, but not infant, rhesus monkeys: field experiments. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  Katja Liebal,et al.  Gestural communication in nonhuman and human primates , 2007 .

[45]  William D Hopkins,et al.  Gesture handedness predicts asymmetry in the chimpanzee inferior frontal gyrus , 2006, Neuroreport.

[46]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The gestural communication of apes , 2007 .

[47]  Michael Fisher,et al.  Tree of Origin , 2002 .

[48]  G. Ettlinger Hand Preference, Ability, Hemispheric Specialization: In How Far are these Factors Related in the Monkey? , 1988, Cortex.

[49]  M. Arbib From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[50]  James Copland,et al.  PROCEEDINGS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY. , 2022 .

[51]  Timothy J. Crow,et al.  Directional asymmetry is the key to the origin of modern Homo sapiens (the Broca‐Annett axiom): A reply to Rogers' review of The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens , 2004 .

[52]  J. H. Dewson 4 – Preliminary Evidence of Hemispheric Asymmetry of Auditory Function in Monkeys , 1977 .

[53]  J. Russell,et al.  Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees: evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal , 2006 .

[54]  Joanna Blake,et al.  Form and function in the development of pointing and reaching gestures , 1994 .

[55]  William D. Hopkins,et al.  Left Hemisphere Specialization for Oro-Facial Movements of Learned Vocal Signals by Captive Chimpanzees , 2008, PloS one.

[56]  E. Ringelstein,et al.  Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  Klaus Zuberbühler,et al.  The Phylogenetic Roots of Language , 2005 .

[58]  J. Vauclair,et al.  Perceptual and Motor Lateralization in Two Species of Baboons , 2007 .

[59]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Lateralized scratching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence of a functional asymmetry during arousal. , 2006, Emotion.

[60]  D. Kimura,et al.  Manual activity during speaking. II. Left-handers. , 1973, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  W. McGrew,et al.  Laterality of limb function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: comprehensive study of spontaneous activities , 1996 .

[62]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[63]  D. Kimura,et al.  Manual activity during speaking. I. Right-handers. , 1973, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  R. Doty,et al.  Lateralization in the nervous system , 1977 .

[65]  W. Hopkins,et al.  Hand preferences for a coordinated bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): cross-sectional analysis. , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.

[66]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Vervet monkeys and humans show brain asymmetries for processing conspecific vocalizations, but with opposite patterns of laterality , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[67]  Karen Emmorey,et al.  The neural correlates of sign versus word production , 2007, NeuroImage.

[68]  C. Heyes,et al.  Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .

[69]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  Inhibition of social behavior in chimpanzees under high‐density conditions , 1997 .

[70]  J. Vauclair Lateralization of communicative signals in nonhuman primates and the hypothesis of the gestural origin of language , 2004 .

[71]  Alex Martin,et al.  Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the macaque , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[72]  W. Hopkins,et al.  Behavioral evidence for the cutaneous expression of emotion in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) , 2004 .

[73]  F. Nottebohm,et al.  Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language [and Comments and Reply] , 1973, Current Anthropology.

[74]  William D Hopkins,et al.  The Distribution and Development of Handedness for Manual Gestures in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2005, Psychological science.

[75]  Alfonso Troisi,et al.  Displacement Activities as a Behavioral Measure of Stress in Nonhuman Primates and Human Subjects , 2002, Stress.

[76]  Kenneth Webb,et al.  Evolution of Communication Simulation of Adaptive Behavior – Project Report , 2004 .

[77]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Language and hand preference in early development , 1986 .

[78]  A. Whiten,et al.  Social anxiety, relationships and self-directed behaviour among wild female olive baboons , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[79]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey's temporal poles , 2004, Nature.

[80]  Virginia Volterra,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for sign language , 1996, Neuropsychologia.