Endogenous sources of variation in language acquisition

Significance Children are exposed to vast quantities of data exhibiting the key structural features of their language. Are these structural features acquired from the data or are they imposed on the data by learners? We identify a piece of grammatical knowledge that is systematic within an individual speaker but varies unpredictably across a population of speakers of ostensibly a single language. Further, parents’ knowledge in this domain does not predict children’s knowledge. The independence of parents’ and children’s knowledge indicates that the relevant grammatical structures are not acquired from experience, but are supplied by learners. This dissociation between the grammatical knowledge of children and their parents demonstrates that children actively construct grammatical knowledge from highly ambiguous evidence. A fundamental question in the study of human language acquisition centers around apportioning explanatory force between the experience of the learner and the core knowledge that allows learners to represent that experience. We provide a previously unidentified kind of data identifying children’s contribution to language acquisition. We identify one aspect of grammar that varies unpredictably across a population of speakers of what is ostensibly a single language. We further demonstrate that the grammatical knowledge of parents and their children is independent. The combination of unpredictable variation and parent–child independence suggests that the relevant structural feature is supplied by each learner independent of experience with the language. This structural feature is abstract because it controls variation in more than one construction. The particular case we examine is the position of the verb in the clause structure of Korean. Because Korean is a head-final language, evidence for the syntactic position of the verb is both rare and indirect. We show that (i) Korean speakers exhibit substantial variability regarding this aspect of the grammar, (ii) this variability is attested between speakers but not within a speaker, (iii) this variability controls interpretation in two surface constructions, and (iv) it is independent in parents and children. According to our findings, when the exposure language is compatible with multiple grammars, learners acquire a single systematic grammar. Our observation that children and their parents vary independently suggests that the choice of grammar is driven in part by a process operating internal to individual learners.

[1]  E. Newport,et al.  When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Jean-Yves Pollock Verb movement, universal grammar and the structure of IP , 1989 .

[3]  S. Kapur,et al.  On the use of triggers in parameter setting , 1996 .

[4]  Melissa Bowerman,et al.  Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: Implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. , 1982 .

[5]  Janet Dean Fodor,et al.  Parsing to Learn , 1998 .

[6]  William Snyder,et al.  On the Nature of Syntactic Variation: Evidence from Complex Predicates and Complex Word-Formation , 2001 .

[7]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Lectures on Government and Binding , 1981 .

[8]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar , 2003, Cognition.

[9]  Chung-hye Han,et al.  An Experimental Investigation into the Syntax of Negation in Japanese , 2008 .

[10]  William Gregory Sakas,et al.  Disambiguating Syntactic Triggers , 2012 .

[11]  Charles D. Yang,et al.  Knowledge and learning in natural language , 2000 .

[12]  Noam Chomsky Knowledge of language: its nature, origin, and use , 1988 .

[13]  Noam Chomsky Reflections on Language. , 1977 .

[14]  J. Yoon Korean verbal inflection and checking theory , 1994 .

[15]  B. Dresher,et al.  A computational learning model for metrical phonology , 1990, Cognition.

[16]  Seok Choong Song On Interpreting the Scope of Negation in Korean , 1982 .

[17]  Kazuyo Otani,et al.  V-raising and VP-ellipsis , 1991 .

[18]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  Statistical insensitivity in the acquisition of Tsez noun classes , 2014 .

[19]  S. Waxman,et al.  What infants know about syntax but couldn't have learned: experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months , 2003, Cognition.

[20]  A. Kroch Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change , 1989, Language Variation and Change.

[21]  Chung-hye Han,et al.  V-Raising and Grammar Competition in Korean: Evidence from Negation and Quantifier Scope , 2007, Linguistic Inquiry.

[22]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  Selective learning in the acquisition of Kannada ditransitives , 2011 .

[23]  R. Thornton,et al.  Adventures in long-distance moving: The acquisition of complex Wh-questions , 1990 .

[24]  Masatoshi Koisumi,et al.  String Vacuous Overt Verb Raising , 2000 .

[25]  S. Crain Language acquisition in the absence of experience , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  Robin Clark,et al.  A Computational Model of Language Learnability and Language Change , 2018, Diachronic and Comparative Syntax.

[27]  D. Hubel,et al.  Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. , 1977, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  M. R. Manzini Learnability and Cognition , 1991 .

[29]  B. Ambridge,et al.  Semantics versus statistics in the retreat from locative overgeneralization errors , 2012, Cognition.

[30]  J. Musolino,et al.  Children's command of quantification , 2002, Cognition.

[31]  A. Senghas,et al.  Children Creating Language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language Acquired a Spatial Grammar , 2001, Psychological science.

[32]  Takuya Goro,et al.  Language -specific constraints on scope interpretation in first language acquisition , 2007 .

[33]  Mark C. Baker,et al.  Mapping the Terrain of Language Learning , 2005 .

[34]  Lisa Pearl,et al.  When Unbiased Probabilistic Learning Is Not Enough: Acquiring a Parametric System of Metrical Phonology , 2011 .

[35]  Lisa Pearl,et al.  Syntactic Islands and Learning Biases: Combining Experimental Syntax and Computational Modeling to Investigate the Language Acquisition Problem , 2013 .

[36]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[37]  P. Marler,et al.  Selective Vocal Learning in a Sparrow , 1977, Science.

[38]  Paul Égré,et al.  Explanation in Linguistics , 2015 .

[39]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  Why children aren't universally successful with quantification , 2006 .

[40]  Paul Hagstrom,et al.  Implications of Child Errors for the Syntax of Negation in Korean , 2002 .

[41]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Gestural communication in deaf children: noneffect of parental input on language development. , 1983, Science.

[42]  E. Newport,et al.  Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  Kenneth Wexler,et al.  Formal Principles of Language Acquisition , 1980 .

[44]  F. Dyer,et al.  Development of sun compensation by honeybees: how partially experienced bees estimate the sun's course. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  S. Pinker Formal models of language learning , 1979, Cognition.

[46]  Jeffrey Lidz,et al.  When Domain-General Learning Fails and When It Succeeds: Identifying the Contribution of Domain Specificity , 2009 .

[47]  Theresia Olsson Neve Knowledge and Learning , 2015 .