Instance-based generalization for human judgments about uncertainty

While previous studies have shown that human behavior adjusts in response to uncertainty, it is still not well understood how uncertainty is estimated and represented. As probability distributions are high dimensional objects, only constrained families of distributions with a low number of parameters can be specified from finite data. However, it is unknown what the structural assumptions are that the brain uses to estimate them. We introduce a novel paradigm that requires human participants of either sex to explicitly estimate the dispersion of a distribution over future observations. Judgments are based on a very small sample from a centered, normally distributed random variable that was suggested by the framing of the task. This probability density estimation task could optimally be solved by inferring the dispersion parameter of a normal distribution. We find that although behavior closely tracks uncertainty on a trial-by-trial basis and resists an explanation with simple heuristics, it is hardly consistent with parametric inference of a normal distribution. Despite the transparency of the simple generating process, participants estimate a distribution biased towards the observed instances while still strongly generalizing beyond the sample. The inferred internal distributions can be well approximated by a nonparametric mixture of spatially extended basis distributions. Thus, our results suggest that fluctuations have an excessive effect on human uncertainty judgments because of representations that can adapt overly flexibly to the sample. This might be of greater utility in more general conditions in structurally uncertain environments.

[1]  Carl E. Rasmussen,et al.  Gaussian processes for machine learning , 2005, Adaptive computation and machine learning.

[2]  A. Pouget,et al.  Probabilistic brains: knowns and unknowns , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian model selection for group studies (vol 46, pg 1005, 2009) , 2009 .

[4]  Charles Kemp,et al.  The discovery of structural form , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  D. Meyer,et al.  Function learning: induction of continuous stimulus-response relations. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Simon Barthelmé,et al.  Flexible mechanisms underlie the evaluation of visual confidence , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  John K. Kruschke,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology: Models of Categorization , 2008 .

[8]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[9]  P. Cz. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .

[10]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  A Tutorial on Bayesian Nonparametric Models , 2011, 1106.2697.

[11]  R. Dolan,et al.  Metacognition: computation, biology and function , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[13]  Joshua I. Sanders,et al.  Signatures of a Statistical Computation in the Human Sense of Confidence , 2016, Neuron.

[14]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The conceptual basis of function learning and extrapolation: Comparison of rule-based and associative-based models , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[16]  J. Bowers,et al.  Bayesian just-so stories in psychology and neuroscience. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Joseph L. Austerweil,et al.  Structure and Flexibility in Bayesian Models of Cognition , 2015 .

[18]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[19]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time , 2014, Neuron.

[20]  R. Shepard,et al.  Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. , 1987, Science.

[21]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Neural coding of uncertainty and probability. , 2014, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  Michael D. Lee,et al.  Sampling Assumptions in Inductive Generalization , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[23]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[24]  Peter E. Latham,et al.  Doubly Bayesian Analysis of Confidence in Perceptual Decision-Making , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[25]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  A rational model of function learning , 2015, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[26]  T. Başar,et al.  A New Approach to Linear Filtering and Prediction Problems , 2001 .

[27]  M. Oaksford,et al.  On the Source of Human Irrationality , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  A detailed comparison of optimality and simplicity in perceptual decision making. , 2016, Psychological review.

[29]  F. Ashby,et al.  Categorization as probability density estimation , 1995 .

[30]  J. Carroll FUNCTIONAL LEARNING: THE LEARNING OF CONTINUOUS FUNCTIONAL MAPPINGS RELATING STIMULUS AND RESPONSE CONTINUA , 1963 .

[31]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Robust averaging during perceptual judgment , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  M. Landy,et al.  Optimal Compensation for Changes in Task-Relevant Movement Variability , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Heuristic decision making. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Multidimensional Models of Perception and Cognition , 2014 .

[35]  Braden A. Purcell,et al.  Hierarchical decision processes that operate over distinct timescales underlie choice and changes in strategy , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  Computational rationality: A converging paradigm for intelligence in brains, minds, and machines , 2015, Science.

[37]  Adam Kepecs,et al.  A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animals , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[38]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian model selection for group studies , 2009, NeuroImage.

[39]  Nathaniel D. Daw,et al.  Human Representation of Visuo-Motor Uncertainty as Mixtures of Orthogonal Basis Distributions , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Probabilistic models of cognition: exploring representations and inductive biases , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  A. Pouget,et al.  Not Noisy, Just Wrong: The Role of Suboptimal Inference in Behavioral Variability , 2012, Neuron.

[42]  Rubén Moreno-Bote,et al.  Decision Confidence and Uncertainty in Diffusion Models with Partially Correlated Neuronal Integrators , 2010, Neural Computation.

[43]  B. Schölkopf,et al.  Generalization and similarity in exemplar models of categorization: Insights from machine learning , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  Peter Bossaerts,et al.  Risk, Unexpected Uncertainty, and Estimation Uncertainty: Bayesian Learning in Unstable Settings , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[45]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Extrapolation: the sine qua non for abstraction in function learning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[47]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Extrapolation: the sine qua non for abstraction in function learning. , 1997 .

[48]  A. Pouget,et al.  The Cost of Accumulating Evidence in Perceptual Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.