Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling

Children take years to learn symbolic arithmetic. Nevertheless, non-human animals, human adults with no formal education, and human infants represent approximate number in arrays of objects and sequences of events, and they use these capacities to perform approximate addition and subtraction. Do children harness these abilities when they begin to learn school mathematics? In two experiments in different schools, kindergarten children from diverse backgrounds were tested on their non-symbolic arithmetic abilities during the school year, as well as on their mastery of number words and symbols. Performance of non-symbolic arithmetic predicted children's mathematics achievement at the end of the school year, independent of achievement in reading or general intelligence. Non-symbolic arithmetic performance was also related to children's mastery of number words and symbols, which figured prominently in the assessments of mathematics achievement in both schools. Thus, non-symbolic and symbolic numerical abilities are specifically related, in children of diverse socio-economic backgrounds, near the start of mathematics instruction.

[1]  R. Case,et al.  Evaluating the breadth and depth of training effects when central conceptual structures are taught. , 2008, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[2]  Jonathan M. Campbell,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test , 2010 .

[3]  Susan C. Levine,et al.  Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. , 1992 .

[4]  Alicia Sandman,et al.  Dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills , 2010 .

[5]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children's development of number comparison. , 2008, Developmental science.

[6]  Selcuk R. Sirin Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review of Research , 2005 .

[7]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[8]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise , 2003 .

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Approximate quantities and exact number words: dissociable systems , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction , 2007, Nature.

[11]  Jesse Snedeker,et al.  When is Four Far More Than Three? Children’s Generalization of Newly-Acquired Number Words , 2009 .

[12]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear numerical board games promotes low-income children's numerical development. , 2008, Developmental science.

[13]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .

[14]  K. McGilly,et al.  Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice. , 1994 .

[15]  Marinella Cappelletti,et al.  rTMS over the intraparietal sulcus disrupts numerosity processing , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[17]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[18]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[19]  F. Nielsen,et al.  The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. , 1995 .

[20]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers , 2005, Cognition.

[21]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[22]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[23]  K. Wynn,et al.  Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.

[24]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. , 1994 .

[26]  G. Dawson,et al.  Human behavior and the developing brain , 1994 .

[27]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.

[28]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  Hilary Barth,et al.  Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[31]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.