Geometric and featural systems, separable and combined: Evidence from reorientation in people with Williams syndrome
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Cheng. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.
[2] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[3] Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg,et al. Genetic Contributions to Human Gyrification: Sulcal Morphometry in Williams Syndrome , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] Paul Koch,et al. Functional, structural, and metabolic abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in Williams syndrome. , 2005, The Journal of clinical investigation.
[5] C. Gallistel,et al. The neuroscience of learning: beyond the Hebbian synapse. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.
[6] I. Sigel,et al. HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .
[7] Juan Pedro Vargas,et al. Hippocampal formation is required for geometric navigation in pigeons , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.
[8] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[9] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[10] Nora S Newcombe,et al. Reorienting When Cues Conflict , 2008, Psychological science.
[11] Christian F. Doeller,et al. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[12] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Stable Panoramic Views Facilitate Snap-Shot Like Memories for Spatial Reorientation in Homing Pigeons , 2011, PloS one.
[13] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Reorientation by geometric and landmark information in environments of different size. , 2005, Developmental science.
[14] Alexandra D. Twyman,et al. Malleability in the development of spatial reorientation. , 2013, Developmental psychobiology.
[15] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: Young children’s use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task , 2012, Cognition.
[16] N. Burgess. Spatial Cognition and the Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[17] Jennifer E Sutton,et al. Spinning in the scanner: neural correlates of virtual reorientation. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[18] Gabriele Janzen,et al. Selective neural representation of objects relevant for navigation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] Christian F. Doeller,et al. The role of landmarks and boundaries in the development of spatial memory. , 2009, Developmental science.
[20] M. D’Esposito,et al. Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[21] Neil Burgess,et al. Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[22] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment , 2007, Animal Cognition.
[23] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[24] E. Spelke,et al. Human Spatial Representation: Insights from Animals , 2002 .
[25] E. Spelke,et al. Inexperienced newborn chicks use geometry to spontaneously reorient to an artificial social partner. , 2015, Developmental science.
[26] Ken Cheng,et al. Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric module , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] Sang Ah Lee,et al. A modular geometric mechanism for reorientation in children , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[28] R. Clark,et al. The medial temporal lobe. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[29] M A Good,et al. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.
[30] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[31] G. Vallortigara,et al. From natural geometry to spatial cognition , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[32] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Navigation by environmental geometry: the use of zebrafish as a model , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[33] Marcia L Spetch,et al. Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: II. Do pigeons (Columba livia) encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? , 2004, Journal of comparative psychology.
[34] Ronald V. Schmelzer,et al. Differential Abilities Scales , 2008 .
[35] L. Hildman,et al. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test , 1993 .
[36] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Reorientation and Landmark-Guided Search by Young Children , 2006, Psychological science.
[37] N. Schneiderman,et al. Delay versus trace heart-rate classical discrimination conditioning in rabbits as a function of interstimulus interval. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[38] Stephan Eliez,et al. IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams Syndrome: A High-Resolution MRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Chicks, like children, spontaneously reorient by three-dimensional environmental geometry, not by image matching , 2012, Biology Letters.
[40] Joseph M. Austen,et al. Transfer of spatial search between environments in human adults and young children (Homo sapiens): implications for representation of local geometry by spatial systems. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.
[41] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Spontaneous Reorientation Is Guided by Perceived Surface Distance, Not by Image Matching Or Comparison , 2012, PloS one.
[42] Barbara Landau,et al. Spatial Representation: From Gene to Mind , 2012 .
[43] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Dissecting the Geometric Module , 2006, Psychological science.
[44] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. Animals' use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space , 2005, Cognition.
[45] Lynn Nadel,et al. Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.
[46] Nora S Newcombe,et al. Why size counts: children's spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. , 2008, Developmental science.
[47] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Development of Spatial Cognition , 2007 .
[48] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.
[49] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Understanding the mapping between numerical approximation and number words: evidence from Williams syndrome and typical development. , 2014, Developmental science.
[50] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Separate processing mechanisms for encoding of geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[51] Russell A. Epstein,et al. Hippocampal size predicts rapid learning of a cognitive map in humans , 2013, Hippocampus.
[52] Janellen Huttenlocher,et al. How toddlers represent enclosed spaces , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[53] Barbara Landau,et al. Impaired geometric reorientation caused by genetic defect , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[54] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Experience and geometry: controlled-rearing studies with chicks , 2010, Animal Cognition.
[55] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Cognitive effects of language on human navigation , 2011, Cognition.
[56] Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg,et al. Neural mechanisms in Williams syndrome: a unique window to genetic influences on cognition and behaviour , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[57] C. Mervis,et al. Neural Basis of Genetically Determined Visuospatial Construction Deficit in Williams Syndrome , 2004, Neuron.
[58] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Using geometry to specify location: implications for spatial coding in children and nonhuman animals , 2007, Psychological research.
[59] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[60] Carolyn B. Mervis,et al. The Williams Syndrome Cognitive Profile , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[61] Alinda Friedman,et al. Penetrating the geometric module: catalyzing children's use of landmarks. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[62] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Children's use of geometry for reorientation. , 2008, Developmental science.
[63] Sang Ah Lee,et al. Young Children Reorient by Computing Layout Geometry, Not by Matching Images of the Environment , 2010 .
[64] Alexandra D. Twyman,et al. Young children's use of features to reorient is more than just associative: further evidence against a modular view of spatial processing. , 2010, Developmental science.
[65] U Bellugi,et al. Evidence for superior parietal impairment in Williams syndrome , 2005, Neurology.
[66] Russell A. Epstein. Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[67] Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al. How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[68] Mark Blades,et al. How do individuals with Williams syndrome learn a route in a real-world environment? , 2010, Developmental science.
[69] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Spatial reorientation by geometry with freestanding objects and extended surfaces: a unifying view , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[70] Almut Hupbach,et al. Reorientation in a rhombic environment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric module , 2005 .
[71] N. Newcombe,et al. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[72] L. Hermer-Vazquez,et al. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks , 2001, Cognition.
[73] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Making Space: The Development of Spatial Representation and Reasoning , 2000 .