Exploring the Cognitive Load of Expert and Novice Map Users Using EEG and Eye Tracking

The main objective of this research is to explore the cognitive processes of expert and novice map users during the retrieval of map-related information, within varying difficulty levels (i.e., easy, moderate, hard), by using eye tracking and electroencephalogram (EEG). In this context, we present a spatial memory experiment consisting of a large number of stimuli to study the effect of task difficulty on map users’ behavior through cognitive load measurements. Next to the reaction time and success rate, we used fixation and saccade related eye tracking metrics (i.e., average fixation duration, the number of fixations per second, saccade amplitude and saccade velocity), and EEG power spectrum (i.e., event-related changes in alpha and theta frequency bands) to identify the cognitive load. While fixation metrics indicated no statistically significant difference between experts and novices, saccade metrics proved the otherwise. EEG power spectral density analysis, on the other side, suggested an increase in theta (i.e., event-related synchronization) and a decrease in alpha (except moderate tasks) (i.e., event-related desynchronization) at all difficulty levels of the task for both experts and novices, which is an indicator of cognitive load. Although no significant difference emerged between two groups, we found a significant difference in their overall performances when the participants were classified as good and relatively bad learners. Triangulating EEG results with the recorded eye tracking data and the qualitative analysis of focus maps indeed provided a detailed insight on the differences of the individuals’ cognitive processes during this spatial memory task.

[1]  Chris Parnin,et al.  Dazed: Measuring the Cognitive Load of Solving Technical Interview Problems at the Whiteboard , 2018, 2018 IEEE/ACM 40th International Conference on Software Engineering: New Ideas and Emerging Technologies Results (ICSE-NIER).

[2]  A. Keil,et al.  Alpha-band activity reflects reduction of mental effort in a comparison task: A source space analysis , 2006, Brain Research.

[3]  Radha Nila Meghanathan,et al.  Refixation patterns reveal memory-encoding strategies in free viewing , 2019, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[4]  M Doppelmayr,et al.  Brain oscillations and human memory: EEG correlates in the upper alpha and theta band , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  Wen-Chin Li,et al.  The Investigation of Visual Attention and Workload by Experts and Novices in the Cockpit , 2013, HCI.

[6]  Mathias Benedek,et al.  Divergent thinking training is related to frontal electroencephalogram alpha synchronization , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Patterns of theta oscillation reflect the neural basis of individual differences in epistemic motivation , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[8]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  How Does Visual Attention Differ Between Experts and Novices on Physics Problems , 2010 .

[9]  Nicolas Debue,et al.  What does germane load mean? An empirical contribution to the cognitive load theory , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[10]  P. Thorndyke,et al.  Individual differences in procedures for knowledge acquisition from maps , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  Thorvald Haerem,et al.  The influence of degree of expertise and objective task complexity on perceived task complexity and performance. , 2007, The Journal of applied psychology.

[12]  Kristien Ooms,et al.  Digital sketch maps and eye tracking statistics as instruments to obtain insights into spatial cognition , 2018, Journal of eye movement research.

[13]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  Effects of Working Memory Load on Visual Selective Attention: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  V. Ibáñez,et al.  Frontal theta event-related synchronization: comparison of directed attention and working memory load effects , 2006, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[15]  P. Maeyer,et al.  Study of the attentive behavior of novice and expert map users using eye tracking , 2014 .

[16]  Leidy J. Castro-Meneses,et al.  Validating theta power as an objective measure of cognitive load in educational video , 2020, Educational Technology Research and Development.

[17]  Adoración Antolí,et al.  Main sequence: an index for detecting mental workload variation in complex tasks. , 2011, Applied ergonomics.

[18]  E. Basar,et al.  Beta oscillatory responses in healthy subjects and subjects with mild cognitive impairment , 2013, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[19]  Ulrike Pfeil,et al.  Measuring Cognitive Load using Eye Tracking Technology in Visual Computing , 2016, BELIV '16.

[20]  Peter W. Kaplan,et al.  Consensus Statement on Continuous EEG in Critically Ill Adults and Children, Part II: Personnel, Technical Specifications, and Clinical Practice , 2015, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[21]  Pavlo D. Antonenko,et al.  Using Electroencephalography to Measure Cognitive Load , 2010 .

[22]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Simultaneous desynchronization and synchronization of different alpha responses in the human electroencephalograph: a neglected paradox? , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[23]  E. Hunt Mechanics of Verbal Ability. , 1978 .

[24]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[25]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[26]  Weihua Dong,et al.  Using Eye Tracking to Explore Differences in Map-Based Spatial Ability between Geographers and Non-Geographers , 2018, ISPRS Int. J. Geo Inf..

[27]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Estimating workload using EEG spectral power and ERPs in the n-back task , 2012, Journal of neural engineering.

[28]  Lenka Havelková,et al.  What Went Wrong for Bad Solvers during Thematic Map Analysis? Lessons Learned from an Eye-Tracking Study , 2020, ISPRS Int. J. Geo Inf..

[29]  Philippe De Maeyer,et al.  EEG & Eye Tracking User Experiments for Spatial Memory Task on Maps , 2019, ISPRS Int. J. Geo Inf..

[30]  K. Pope,et al.  Cognitive tasks augment gamma EEG power , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[31]  Hosam Al-Samarraie,et al.  Effects of map design characteristics on users' search performance and cognitive load , 2019, Electron. Libr..

[32]  Frank Witlox,et al.  Interpreting maps through the eyes of expert and novice users , 2012, Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci..

[33]  Yolanda Postigo,et al.  The Learning of a Geographical Map by Experts and Novices , 1998 .

[34]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[35]  O. Jensen,et al.  Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.