An Antidote to the Imager's Fallacy, or How to Identify Brain Areas That Are in Limbo

Traditionally, fMRI data are analyzed using statistical parametric mapping approaches. Regardless of the precise thresholding procedure, these approaches ultimately divide the brain in regions that do or do not differ significantly across experimental conditions. This binary classification scheme fosters the so-called imager's fallacy, where researchers prematurely conclude that region A is selectively involved in a certain cognitive task because activity in that region reaches statistical significance and activity in region B does not. For such a conclusion to be statistically valid, however, a test on the differences in activation across these two regions is required. Here we propose a simple GLM-based method that defines an “in-between” category of brain regions that are neither significantly active nor inactive, but rather “in limbo”. For regions that are in limbo, the activation pattern is inconclusive: it does not differ significantly from baseline, but neither does it differ significantly from regions that do show significant changes from baseline. This pattern indicates that measurement was insufficiently precise. By directly testing differences in activation, our procedure helps reduce the impact of the imager's fallacy. The method is illustrated using concrete examples.

[1]  Lourens J. Waldorp,et al.  Robust and Unbiased Variance of GLM Coefficients for Misspecified Autocorrelation and Hemodynamic Response Models in fMRI , 2009, Int. J. Biomed. Imaging.

[2]  Kamel Hamrouni,et al.  A Based Bayesian Wavelet Thresholding Method to Enhance Nuclear Imaging , 2009, Int. J. Biomed. Imaging.

[3]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A General Statistical Analysis for fMRI Data , 2000, NeuroImage.

[4]  TA Hare,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[5]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[6]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review , 2003, Statistical methods in medical research.

[7]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Broca's Area and the Hierarchical Organization of Human Behavior , 2006, Neuron.

[8]  A. Gelman,et al.  The Difference Between “Significant” and “Not Significant” is not Itself Statistically Significant , 2006 .

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al.  Nipype: A Flexible, Lightweight and Extensible Neuroimaging Data Processing Framework in Python , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[12]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Handbook of Functional MRI Data Analysis: Index , 2011 .

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  To Smooth or Not to Smooth? Bias and Efficiency in fMRI Time-Series Analysis , 2000, NeuroImage.

[15]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Tools of the trade: psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[16]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Information-based functional brain mapping. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Martin Petri,et al.  To Smooth or Not to Smooth - the Impact of Grants and Remittances on the Equilibrium Real Exchange Rate in Jordan , 2006, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[18]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Handbook of Functional MRI Data Analysis: Modeling brain connectivity , 2011 .

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[20]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[21]  Yves Rosseel,et al.  Simulating fMRI data: the R package neuRosim , 2011 .

[22]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Modeling group fMRI data. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[24]  Noah D. Brenowitz,et al.  Whole-brain, time-locked activation with simple tasks revealed using massive averaging and model-free analysis , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  R. Henson What can Functional Neuroimaging Tell the Experimental Psychologist? , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[27]  Andrea Bergmann,et al.  Statistical Parametric Mapping The Analysis Of Functional Brain Images , 2016 .