Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture

We investigated the effect of unseen hand posture on cross-modal, visuo-tactile links in covert spatial attention. In Experiment 1, a spatially nonpredictive visual cue was presented to the left or right hemifield shortly before a tactile target on either hand. To examine the spatial coordinates of any crossmodal cuing, the unseen hands were either uncrossed or crossed so that the left hand lay to the right and vice versa. Tactile up/down (i.e., index finger/thumb) judgments were better on the same side of external space as the visual cue, for both crossed and uncrossed postures. Thus, which hand was advantaged by a visual cue in a particular hemifield reversed across the different unseen postures. In Experiment 2, nonpredictive tactile cues now preceded visual targets. Up/down judgments for the latter were better on the same side of external space as the tactile cue, again for both postures. These results demonstrate cross-modal links between vision and touch in exogenous covert spatial attention that remap across changes in unseen hand posture, suggesting a modulatory role for proprioception.

[1]  R. Wallace,et al.  S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  R. Wallace Spatiel S-R compatibility effects involving kinesthetic cues. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  M. Posner,et al.  On the selection of signals , 1973, Memory & cognition.

[4]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual signal detection and the locus of foreperiod effects , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[5]  M. Kinsbourne The mechanism of hemispheric control of the lateral gradient of attention , 1975 .

[6]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  Juhani Hyva¨rinen Regional distribution of functions in parietal association area 7 of the monkey , 1981, Brain Research.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  J Hyvärinen,et al.  Regional distribution of functions in parietal association area 7 of the monkey. , 1981, Brain research.

[11]  Giacomo Rizzolatti,et al.  Evidence of interhemispheric transmission in laterality effects , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  John E. Laird,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1986 .

[13]  L. G. Gawryszewski,et al.  What is crossed in crossed-hand effects? , 1986 .

[14]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[15]  C. Butter Varieties of Attention and Disturbances of Attention: A Neuropsychological Analysis , 1987 .

[16]  M. Jeannerod Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .

[17]  M. Kinsbourne Mechanisms of Unilateral Neglect , 1987 .

[18]  H. Buchtel,et al.  Spatial attentional shifts: Further evidence for the role of polysensory mechanisms using visual and tactile stimuli , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Steve Steiner,et al.  Missing link , 1989 .

[20]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[21]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Extrageniculate Contributions to Reflex Visual Orienting in Normal Humans: A Temporal Hemifield Advantage , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Effects of Expectancy and Attention in Vibrotactile Choice Reaction Time Tasks , 1992 .

[23]  B. Stein,et al.  The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .

[24]  Jon Driver,et al.  Covert Spatial Orienting in Audition: Exogenous and Endogenous Mechanisms , 1994 .

[25]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[26]  D L Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. III. Contributions of the superior colliculus. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  C. Gross,et al.  The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons , 1995 .

[28]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[29]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. II. Contributions of parietal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[31]  R. Daroff,et al.  Clinical Neurology for Psychiatrists , 1995, Neurology.

[32]  J M Groh,et al.  Saccades to somatosensory targets. III. eye-position-dependent somatosensory activity in primate superior colliculus. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Information integration in perception and communication , 1996 .

[34]  Cj Spence,et al.  Audiovisual links in endogenous covert orienting. , 1996 .

[35]  C. Gross,et al.  Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  A. Treisman,et al.  The line-motion illusion: attention or impletion? , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Elisabetta Làdavas,et al.  Seeing where your hands are , 1997, Nature.

[39]  Paul E. Downing,et al.  The line-motion illusion : attention or impletion? , 1997 .

[40]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Giuseppe di Pellegrino,et al.  Neuropsychological Evidence of an Integrated Visuotactile Representation of Peripersonal Space in Humans , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  C. Spence,et al.  Cross-modal links in exogenous covert spatial orienting between touch, audition, and vision , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  André Dufour,et al.  Importance of attentional mechanisms in audiovisual links , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  C. Frith,et al.  Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. , 2000, Science.

[45]  M. Keeling,et al.  Involuntary orienting to sound improves visual perception , 2022 .

[46]  C. Spence,et al.  Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  L M Ward,et al.  Involuntary Listening Aids Seeing: Evidence From Human Electrophysiology , 2000, Psychological science.

[48]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for the "missing link" in crossmodal attention. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[49]  S. Charles,et al.  10 - Crossmodal Attentional Capture: A Controversy Resolved? , 2001 .

[50]  C. Spence,et al.  Tactile-Visual Links in Exogenous Spatial Attention under Different Postures: Convergent Evidence from Psychophysics and ERPs , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  C. Gross,et al.  A bimodal map of space: somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Spatial S-R compatibility effects in an intentional imitation task , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.