Detached and distracted: ERP correlates of altered attentional function in depersonalisation

[1]  Dong Yang,et al.  Contextual Valence and Sociality Jointly Influence the Early and Later Stages of Neutral Face Processing , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[2]  M. Beutel,et al.  Is that me in the mirror? Depersonalisation modulates tactile mirroring mechanisms , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Steven C. R. Williams,et al.  Cognitive load and autonomic response patterns under negative priming demand in depersonalization‐derealization disorder , 2016, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  S. MacDonald,et al.  Attention capacity and self-report of subjective cognitive decline: A P3 ERP study , 2014, Biological Psychology.

[5]  Manfred E. Beutel,et al.  Altered orientation of spatial attention in depersonalization disorder , 2014, Psychiatry Research.

[6]  Mike Anderson,et al.  Predictive validity of the N2 and P3 ERP components to executive functioning in children: a latent-variable analysis , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[7]  P. Philippot,et al.  Fear of negative evaluation and attentional bias for facial expressions: An event-related study , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  Ewout H. Meijer,et al.  Early emotional processing deficits in depersonalization: An exploration with event-related potentials in an undergraduate sample , 2013, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[9]  P. Philippot,et al.  The impact of the stimulus features and task instructions on facial processing in social anxiety: An ERP investigation , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[10]  Ryuji Takeya,et al.  Time course of spatial and feature selective attention for partly-occluded objects , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  A. David,et al.  Prevalence and childhood antecedents of depersonalization syndrome in a UK birth cohort , 2012, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.

[13]  R. Lewis‐Fernández,et al.  Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. , 2011, Depression and anxiety.

[14]  R. Lewis‐Fernández,et al.  Dissociative disorders in DSM‐5 , 2011, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[15]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Right temporal-parietal junction engagement during spatial reorienting does not depend on strategic attention control , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Shift of attention to the body location of distracters is mediated by perceptual load in sustained somatosensory attention , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[17]  W. Miltner,et al.  Interpretive bias in social phobia: An ERP study with morphed emotional schematic faces , 2009 .

[18]  T. Giesbrecht,et al.  Cognitive Functioning in Depersonalization Disorder , 2007, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[19]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Faster, more intense! The relation between electrophysiological reflections of attentional orienting, sensory gain control, and speed of responding , 2007, Brain Research.

[20]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  M. Beutel,et al.  Depersonalization, Mindfulness, and Childhood Trauma , 2007, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[22]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Interstimulus interval and the selective-attention effect on auditory ERPs: "N1 enhancement" versus processing negativity. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[23]  N. Fox,et al.  Behavioral and Electrophysiological Markers of Selective Attention in Children of Parents with a History of Depression , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[24]  Thomas M. Spalek,et al.  Attentional blink and attentional capture: Endogenous versus exogenous control over paying attention to two important events in close succession , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  D. Linden The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[26]  Dominique Lamy,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety: A behavioral and ERP study , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[27]  G. Berríos,et al.  Die Erfassung des Depersonalisations-Derealisations-Syndroms mit der deutschen Version der Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale (CDS) , 2004 .

[28]  M. Phillips,et al.  Depersonalisation disorder: a cognitive-behavioural conceptualisation. , 2003, Behaviour research and therapy.

[29]  D. Simeon,et al.  Feeling unreal: a depersonalization disorder update of 117 cases. , 2003, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[30]  M. Eimer,et al.  Modulations of early somatosensory ERP components by transient and sustained spatial attention , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Mary L. Phillips,et al.  Depersonalisation disorder: clinical features of 204 cases , 2003, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[32]  Antigona Martínez,et al.  Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  Margot J. Taylor Non-spatial attentional effects on P1 , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[37]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[38]  M. Phillips,et al.  Primary and secondary depersonalisation disorder: a psychometric study. , 2001, Journal of affective disorders.

[39]  G. Woodman,et al.  Event-related potential studies of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  M S Buchsbaum,et al.  Feeling unreal: a PET study of depersonalization disorder. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[41]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Concurrent recording of steady-state and transient event-related potentials as indices of visual-spatial selective attention , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[42]  G. Berríos,et al.  The Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale: a new instrument for the measurement of depersonalisation , 2000, Psychiatry Research.

[43]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[44]  E. Schröger,et al.  ERP effects of intermodal attention and cross-modal links in spatial attention. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[45]  J. Richards,et al.  Localizing the development of covert attention in infants with scalp event-related potentials. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[46]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  Dan J Stein,et al.  Feeling unreal: 30 cases of DSM-III-R depersonalization disorder. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[48]  F. Karayanidis,et al.  Frontal processing negativity in a visual selective attention task. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  E. Schröger,et al.  The location of preceding stimuli affects selective processing in a sustained attention situation. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[50]  S J Luck,et al.  Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  S J Luck,et al.  Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. I. Evidence for early selection. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[53]  Robert Freedman,et al.  Sensory processing dysfunction in the personal experience and neuronal machinery of schizophrenia. , 2015, The American journal of psychiatry.

[54]  Sabrina Pitzalis,et al.  EEG–fMRI Combination for the Study of Visual Perception and Spatial Attention , 2014 .

[55]  M. Eimer The time course of spatial attention: Insights from event-related brain potentials , 2011 .

[56]  J. Polich Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .

[57]  H. Buchner,et al.  7 Kognitive Potenziale (ereigniskorrelierte Potenziale, EKP) , 2005 .

[58]  H. Buchner,et al.  Evozierte Potenziale, Neurovegetative Diagnostik, Okulographie: Methodik und klinische Anwendungen , 2005 .

[59]  A. David,et al.  The epidemiology of depersonalisation and derealisation , 2004, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology.

[60]  J. Schmeidler,et al.  Feeling unreal: cognitive processes in depersonalization. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[61]  G. Mangun Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[62]  S. Luck,et al.  The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.

[63]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[64]  D. Trueman Depersonalization in a nonclinical population. , 1984, The Journal of psychology.

[65]  M. Kutas,et al.  Electrophysiology of cognitive processing. , 1983, Annual review of psychology.