Fusion of Visual and Auditory Stimuli during Saccades: A Bayesian Explanation for Perisaccadic Distortions

Brief stimuli presented near the onset of saccades are grossly mislocalized in space. In this study, we investigated whether the Bayesian hypothesis of optimal sensory fusion could account for the mislocalization. We required subjects to localize visual, auditory, and audiovisual stimuli at the time of saccades (compared with an earlier presented target). During fixation, vision dominates and spatially “captures” the auditory stimulus (the ventriloquist effect). But for perisaccadic presentations, auditory localization becomes more important, so the mislocalized visual stimulus is seen closer to its veridical position. The precision of the bimodal localization (as measured by localization thresholds or just-noticeable difference) was better than either the visual or acoustic stimulus presented in isolation. Both the perceived position of the bimodal stimuli and the improved precision were well predicted by assuming statistically optimal Bayesian-like combination of visual and auditory signals. Furthermore, the time course of localization was well predicted by the Bayesian approach. We present a detailed model that simulates the time-course data, assuming that perceived position is given by the sum of retinal position and a sluggish noisy eye-position signal, obtained by integrating optimally the output of two populations of neural activity: one centered at the current point of gaze, the other centered at the future point of gaze.

[1]  D. Knill,et al.  Apparent surface curvature affects lightness perception , 1991, Nature.

[2]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Christopher J Bockisch,et al.  Different motor systems use similar damped extraretinal eye position information , 1999, Vision Research.

[4]  James J. Clark,et al.  Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems , 1990 .

[5]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[6]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  A P Batista,et al.  Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. , 1999, Science.

[8]  Ilana B. Witten,et al.  Why Seeing Is Believing: Merging Auditory and Visual Worlds , 2005, Neuron.

[9]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Jordan Pola,et al.  Models of the mechanism underlying perceived location of a perisaccadic flash , 2004, Vision Research.

[11]  Joyce Vliegen,et al.  Dynamic Sound Localization during Rapid Eye-Head Gaze Shifts , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  T. Bayes An essay towards solving a problem in the doctrine of chances , 2003 .

[13]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.

[14]  O. Braddick Visual psychophysics , 1997, Current Biology.

[15]  L. Harris,et al.  Auditory Stimulus Detection is Not Suppressed during Saccadic Eye Movements , 1996, Perception.

[16]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex , 2006, Nature.

[18]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[20]  C. Koch,et al.  Attention activates winner-take-all competition among visual filters , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[22]  P Dassonville,et al.  Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Color and Luminance Contrasts Attract Independent Attention , 2002, Current Biology.

[24]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[27]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[28]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[29]  S. Mateeff Saccadic eye movements and localization of visual stimuli , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  J. M. Miller,et al.  Egocentric localization of a perisaccadic flash by manual pointing , 1996, Vision Research.

[32]  A. Pouget,et al.  Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  C L Colby,et al.  Visual, saccade-related, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey extrastriate area V3A. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.

[36]  E Kowler,et al.  Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Nature.