Proactive, but Not Reactive, Distractor Filtering Relies on Local Modulation of Alpha Oscillatory Activity

Filter mechanisms that prevent irrelevant information from consuming the limited storage capacity of visual STM are critical for goal-directed behavior. Alpha oscillatory activity has been related to proactive filtering of anticipated distraction. Yet, distraction in everyday life is not always anticipated, necessitating rapid, reactive filtering mechanisms. Currently, the oscillatory mechanisms underlying reactive distractor filtering remain unclear. In the current EEG study, we investigated whether reactive filtering of distractors also relies on alpha-band oscillatory mechanisms and explored possible contributions by oscillations in other frequency bands. To this end, participants performed a lateralized change detection task in which a varying and unpredicted number of distractors were presented both in the relevant hemifield, among targets, and in the irrelevant hemifield. Results showed that, whereas proactive distractor filtering was accompanied by lateralization of alpha-band activity over posterior scalp regions, reactive distractor filtering was not associated with modulations of oscillatory power in any frequency band. Yet, behavioral and post hoc ERP analyses clearly showed that participants selectively encoded relevant information. On the basis of these results, we conclude that reactive distractor filtering may not be realized through local modulation of alpha-band oscillatory activity.

[1]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Orchestrating Proactive and Reactive Mechanisms for Filtering Distracting Information: Brain-Behavior Relationships Revealed by a Mixed-Design fMRI Study , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[3]  A. Karim,et al.  Brain Oscillatory Substrates of Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity , 2009, Current Biology.

[4]  E. Vogel,et al.  Human Variation in Overriding Attentional Capture , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Ben M. Crittenden,et al.  Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  P. Fries Rhythms for Cognition: Communication through Coherence , 2015, Neuron.

[7]  T. Womelsdorf,et al.  Long-Range Attention Networks: Circuit Motifs Underlying Endogenously Controlled Stimulus Selection , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  H Pashler,et al.  Familiarity and visual change detection , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[10]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  The costly filtering of potential distraction: evidence for a supramodal mechanism. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Amishi P. Jha,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in resolving distractor interference , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Paul Tiesinga,et al.  Oscillatory mechanisms of feedforward and feedback visual processing , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Alpha/Beta and Gamma Oscillations Reflect Functionally Distinct Processes , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Bart Gips,et al.  Temporal coding organized by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing , 2014, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  A. Engel,et al.  Neuronal Synchronization along the Dorsal Visual Pathway Reflects the Focus of Spatial Attention , 2008, Neuron.

[18]  S. Kastner,et al.  Attention in the real world: toward understanding its neural basis , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  O. Jensen,et al.  Rhythmic Pulsing: Linking Ongoing Brain Activity with Evoked Responses , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[20]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Alpha power indexes task-related networks on large and small scales: A multimodal ECoG study in humans and a non-human primate , 2016, NeuroImage.

[22]  T. Heskes,et al.  Covert attention allows for continuous control of brain–computer interfaces , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  Nicholas E. Myers,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Attention during Encoding versus Maintenance of Working Memory: Complementary Views from Event-related Potentials and Alpha-band Oscillations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Single-Trial Regression Elucidates the Role of Prefrontal Theta Oscillations in Response Conflict , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[25]  T. Womelsdorf,et al.  The role of neuronal synchronization in selective attention , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[26]  P. Nunez,et al.  EEG and MEG coherence: Measures of functional connectivity at distinct spatial scales of neocortical dynamics , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[27]  G. Thut,et al.  Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  J. Palva,et al.  Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Correlate with the Successful Inhibition of Unattended Stimuli , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Neural Suppression of Irrelevant Information Underlies Optimal Working Memory Performance , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  H. Karnath,et al.  Keeping Memory Clear and Stable—The Contribution of Human Basal Ganglia and Prefrontal Cortex to Working Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Attention-modulated Alpha-band Oscillations Protect against Intrusion of Irrelevant Information , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Guillaume A. Rousselet,et al.  Quantifying the Time Course of Visual Object Processing Using ERPs: It's Time to Up the Game , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[36]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Matthew C. Keller,et al.  Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression , 2005, NeuroImage.

[38]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  Pulsed Out of Awareness: EEG Alpha Oscillations Represent a Pulsed-Inhibition of Ongoing Cortical Processing , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[39]  Roshan Cools,et al.  Region-specific modulations in oscillatory alpha activity serve to facilitate processing in the visual and auditory modalities , 2014, NeuroImage.

[40]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[41]  U. Mayr,et al.  Switching between filter settings reduces the efficient utilization of visual working memory , 2016, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[42]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[43]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[44]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Anticipatory Attentional Suppression of Visual Features Indexed by Oscillatory Alpha-Band Power Increases:A High-Density Electrical Mapping Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Alexa B. Roggeveen,et al.  Large-scale gamma-band phase synchronization and selective attention. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  Nick F. Ramsey,et al.  Frequency specific spatial interactions in human electrocorticography: V1 alpha oscillations reflect surround suppression , 2013, NeuroImage.

[47]  E. Vogel,et al.  Individual Differences in Recovery Time From Attentional Capture , 2011, Psychological science.

[48]  G. F. Tremblay,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex , 1989, Neurology.

[49]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Intercommunication Between Prefrontal and Posterior Brain Regions for Protecting Visual Working Memory From Distractor Interference , 2014, Psychological science.

[50]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Gamma Activity Coupled to Alpha Phase as a Mechanism for Top-Down Controlled Gating , 2015, PloS one.

[51]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms of Distractor Suppression , 2014 .

[52]  Eric Maris,et al.  Attentional modulations of somatosensory alpha, beta and gamma oscillations dissociate between anticipation and stimulus processing , 2014, NeuroImage.

[53]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[54]  O. Jensen,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against Anticipated Distracters , 2012, Current Biology.

[55]  Tomas Knapen,et al.  Interregional alpha-band synchrony supports temporal cross-modal integration , 2014, NeuroImage.

[56]  Edward Awh,et al.  The bouncer in the brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Marina Schmid,et al.  An Introduction To The Event Related Potential Technique , 2016 .

[58]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Immature Processing Capacity and Filtering in Visuospatial Working Memory in Adolescents , 2012, PloS one.

[59]  Julia Kastner,et al.  Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis Testing , 2005 .

[60]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.